M. Zitzmann and E. Nieschlag


Institute of Reproductive Medicine of the University, D-48129 Münster, Germany.


Correspondence should be addressed to E Nieschlag, Institute of Reproductive Medicine of the University, Domagkstr 11, D-48129 Münster, Germany; E-mail: nieschl@uni-muenster.de.


Abstract

This review summarises the correlations between testosterone levels and male physical appearance and behaviour. Methodological shortcomings concerning the measurement of testosterone could limit the value of these findings. In addition, testosterone measured in body fluids represents only one step in the cascade of action from production to biological effect, and could therefore provide only a limited view of the complexity of physiological events. Testosterone levels are influenced by conditions that are partly controlled or initiated by the hormone itself, but also by circumstances beyond hormonal or individual control. Different kinds of behaviour are not only subject to influence by environment, but also androgens can reinforce the particular kind of conduct and the behavioural impact can wield negative or positive feedback on testosterone secretion. Therefore, both generalisation and individualisation of study results will lead to doubtful conclusions and prejudices. Results of such studies must be viewed with caution, and over-simplification as well as over-interpretation should be avoided.



Introduction


Testosterone is the best known hormone outside professional medicine. The popularity of the male sex hormone derives from the fact that it acts in all organs and systems and has a significant influence on important aspects of life such as physical appearance, behaviour, mentality, abilities, sexuality and social status. The media are focussed on testosterone and while they disseminate much information, they also indulge in speculations on its effects, as highlighted by the following quotation: ‘the He hormone...has become a metaphor of manhood.it affects every aspect of our society, from high divorce rates and adolescent male violence to exploding cults of bodybuilding.’ (1). Often scientific publications reinforce these speculations by finding yet another connection between high or low testosterone levels and physical or mental variables. However, because man consists of more than unrelated components, the question arises whether the onedimensional ups and downs of serum testosterone levels are sufficient to explain the complexities of (hu)man life and whether the supposed/alleged connections may not be oversimplifying and misleading when generalised or applied to healthy eugonadal individuals not exposed to exogenous testosterone. The multitude of associations of low or high testosterone levels with behavioural and physical characteristics makes one wonder whether one hormone can be responsible for so many features and facets of life. In the current article we will review these associations as described in healthy men based on representative and, as far as possible, controlled studies, and point out methodological pitfalls and deficiencies in interpreting results.



Association of endogenous testosterone levels with physical exercise


There are numerous reports on the effects of physical exercise on testosterone levels and vice versa. Many of these studies lack control groups, have very low numbers of participants or combine different effectors on hormone levels. Moreover, a distinction has to be made between physical exercises requiring endurance and those that train strength.


Endurance training/physical stress


Endurance training can have a prolonged effect on androgen levels. Controlled trials involving men under-going endurance training and control groups of sedentary men give the impression of generally lowered androgen levels in exercising men. Normal feedback regulation would require luteinizing hormone (LH) levels to rise with falling testosterone levels. A suppression in the regulatory axis could explain that differences in gonadotropin levels are rarely seen in exercising vs. sedentary men despite marked differences in testosterone levels (2-7).


That training and competition in physical endurance also means exposure to physical stress is shown in controlled settings observing male participants of ultra-marathon competitions. Testosterone levels decrease during contests, whereas LH levels do not change. In addition, cortisol levels, as a prominent endocrine marker for physical or mental stress exposure, are elevated significantly in runners in comparison to controls (8). This is confirmed by other studies on stress and overtraining (9-12).


Patterns of the mental/psychological and physical stress response of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis are the same within one individual. Differential reactivity is seen between so-called high and low responders (13). This may apply to the HPA too; the lowering effect of endurance training on testosterone levels may be seen as a part of a general response pattern to stress in an individual. As described below, mental stress has a negative impact on testosterone secretion. In settings combining the mental and physical aspects of stress, testosterone can drop to clearly hypogonadal levels (14, 15). The decrease of testosterone levels under stressful situations is usually not sufficiently answered by the pituitary to compensate for the decrease.


Strength training


In men, muscle mass and strength are often described as being associated with testosterone levels. This applies to older men as well as to adolescents (16, 17). Strength training can have an acute effect on endocrine functions. Measurements immediately and 5 min post-exercise show an age-dependent increase in testosterone levels (18-22). Persons continuously involved in strength training, however, do not show significant changes in testosterone levels (20, 23, 24). Overtraining as a physical stress factor may decrease androgen levels. Abuse of exogenous testosterone for anabolic effects can also affect other parameters and will not be discussed further (22, 25-29).


Mental stress


The release of cortisol by activation of the HPA axis as a reaction to mental stress is well documented (23). Similarly, stress responses by the HPA axis are also seen. This applies not only to physical stress but also to psychologically disturbing events.


Stressful situations as experienced during work, before tournaments or anticipating exams have been shown to decrease testosterone levels (30-36). Stress release, on the contrary, can have an elevating effect on androgen levels, which is demonstrated by a controlled study involving volunteers practising transcendental meditation (3 7). The same effect was seen in men undergoing workplace reorganisation and threatened by unemployment. After the workplace situation changed for the better, testosterone levels clearly rose; however, there was a marked variation between subjects (38).


It remains unclear whether the drop of testosterone levels in exposure to mental stress is caused by decreased LH secretion or whether an adequate response at the pituitary level is not present. An additional impact factor might be the increased glucocorticoid secretion observed in stressful situations (due to increased corticotropin-releasing hormone production), which may be responsible for down- regulated testosterone biosynthesis in the Leydig cell (39).


Aggression


Aggression is one of the aspects of human behaviour that is often linked to testosterone levels; knowledge and assumptions are often derived from animal models. In a representative study, ten cynomolgus monkeys received injections of either testosterone propionate or a sham solution. Testosterone administration led to a significant increase in aggression, which was mediated by social status. Dominant animals were much more likely to present aggressive behaviour than subordinate ones (40).


In male prison inmates selected for high levels of aggressiveness, testosterone levels have often been described as being linked with violent behaviour (4147), whereas correlations between testosterone and aggression were low when hostility inventories were applied in volunteers who had not been in conflict with the law (48, 49). This contradiction was explained by the fact that physically violent behaviour leading to legal prosecution is likely to occur in a desolate socioeconomic environment and can itself induce testosterone secretion (and thus result in a selection bias for many studies involving prison inmates). The induction of testosterone secretion by external stimuli of aggression should be seen to be dependent on additional factors, such as culture and education; social background can influence these reactions, as shown in students from the northern and southern parts of the USA, the latter describing themselves as having grown up in a ‘culture of honour’ in which insults diminish a man’s reputation. When the volunteers were subjected to verbal insult, the Southerners reacted with significantly more aggressive and dominant behaviour, which was correlated with their rising serum testosterone levels (50). In a socially homogeneous group of German volunteers, levels of saliva testosterone did not change during exposure to aggressive and violent films (51). It is possible that an environment constantly provoking aggressive behaviour may lead to various reactions and qualities of aggressive responses, which in turn may lead to differential hormonal secretion patterns (52).


There are strong indications that there is an interdependent feedback mechanism between testosterone and aggression that is modified by experiences of victory and defeat (see below), as well as by education, cultural and socioeconomic background. It seems that the subject’s own behaviour can cause and reinforce the type of hormonal activity that stimulated the behaviour in the first place. This pattern of positive feedback seems to be necessary to maintain an emotionally stable personality. Emotional instability was found to be related to a greater variability in testosterone secretion (53).


The immense variety of individual response patterns to androgens is demonstrated by a controlled trial in which exceptionally high doses (600 mg/week) of testosterone cypionate were administered; maniac effects were reported in only 16% of the men. The psychological behaviour of the others remained unremarkable (54). Effects of external administration of testosterone on aggressive behaviour are controversial (55-58).


Depressive illness


In hypogonadal men whose often lethargic or depressive mood significantly improved under testosterone therapy (59), studies exploring the relationship between gonadal function and depressive episodes showed that testosterone secretion as well as mean levels were decreased significantly in patients (60, 61). The relationship of major depression to stress becomes evident by the increased baseline activity of the HPA axis in patients with the disease. Urinary cortisol secretion is significantly increased in depressive patients. This phenomenon is also observed in posttraumatic stress disorder, which can lead to depressive states (62, 63). Decreased testosterone levels in depressive illness can be seen as a permanently downregulated secretion, partially maintaining a state of mood that initiated it in the first place. The effects of depressive illness on androgen levels and vice versa are probably closely related to general stress reactions. The mutual character of the feedback mechanism found for testosterone and behaviour may also become evident in this aspect. External administration of testosterone is therefore considered for the treatment of depressive states (64, 65).


Cognitive abilities


Gender differences in cognitive function tests have been widely reported: men tend to excel in fields of spatial cognition, whereas women show greater abilities in verbal fluency (66, 67). Many factors influence the development of these abilities, such as environment, education, cultural background and inherited factors, but sex hormones also play a role in development and maintenance of cognitive functions. Variation and overlapping scores between genders show the limited extent of this impact; to a much higher degree, an individual’s cognitive abilities are derived from experience, education and inheritance (68).


Visuo-spatial cognition


Gender differences in spatial cognition have been described frequently; males excel especially in tasks of mental rotation (69-73). Compared with women, a stronger cerebral laterization concerning various tasks has been observed (74). Right hemisphere specificity in mental rotation procedures (75, 76) can explain the observed difference between genders in such tasks (69, 72). This is also the case in children of both sexes, suggesting an influence of differential foetal sex hormone levels on the formation of brain architecture (70). In healthy men, positive associations of testosterone levels with scores achieved in such tests have been reported (77, 78), but a curvilinear relationship between androgen levels and spatial cognition in humans has also been discussed (79) and some studies report negative relationships (80, 81). An association between the ultradian rhythms of gonadotropin and androgen secretion and performance levels of spatial cognition seems to exist (82).


Only drastically altered testosterone levels yield a coherent picture. In female-to-male transsexuals testosterone has an enhancing, not quickly reversible, effect on spatial ability performance; an opposite effect is reported for male-to-female transsexuals receiving androgen-ablation (83). In hypogonadal men, visuo- spatial abilities are impaired and most studies report improvement during androgen substitution (84-88). Cerebral neuroimaging suggests that this improvement is due to an androgen-mediated activating effect on cerebral structures involved in evaluating data of visuo- spatial content, such as the ventral visual processing stream (89). As long as subjects are not clearly hypogonadal, the learning effects of such tasks are likely to outweigh fluctuations in androgen levels.


Verbal skills


Indications are that testosterone levels seem to be negatively correlated to verbal skills in males (88). Testosterone substitution in elderly males can block the practising effect in verbal fluency (90). Female-to-male transsexuals show a steady decline in verbal fluency under testosterone administration (83). Corresponding effects were seen in male-to-female transsexuals receiving oestrogen treatment; their verbal skills increased in comparison to controls waiting for hormone treatment (91). It has also been demonstrated that oestrogens can enhance verbal skills in postmenopausal women (92). In healthy elderly men, oestrogen levels are positively associated with verbal abilities (93).


Sex hormone-dependent brain formation processes take place during the prenatal period. In later life, androgens and estrogens seem to be able, and necessary, to activate neural pathways established earlier (94). This refers to the distinction between the states of eu- and hypogonadism, but the effects of shifting hormone levels within the eugonadal range are not distinguishable in individuals.



Musical skills


Empirical data concerning musical skills and testosterone levels are presented in an evaluation of 117 adults and 120 adolescents. Musical abilities were enhanced in men with low testosterone levels and in women with high testosterone levels relative to normal ranges (95). Because many factors influence hormone levels, it is difficult to assign an optimal testosterone range (which would have to be maintained) for the development of musical abilities (96).


Social status


Psychoendocrinological aspects of an individual’s social status can be approached by focussing either on hormonal influences on the regulation of social behaviour or vice versa. Social interaction may elicit different activation pathways depending on former experiences of the respective individual. Rank in primates has been extensively studied. For example, in mandrills and squirrel monkeys, social rank is said to be correlated with testosterone levels. In squirrel monkeys, external testosterone cannot change rank- specific behaviour (97, 98). Regarding man, studies involving non-primate mammals are of restricted value, because these animals are often subject to seasonal influences.


In boys, a positive association of testosterone with social dominance as assessed by peer-rating was described. Inter-individual variation was high in these samples (99, 100). An occupation involving high demands and low decision latitude, indicating lower social status, was found to be associated with lower testosterone levels (waiters compared with physicians and air traffic controllers). Variable degrees of stress as a lowering vector of testosterone levels (see above) should be taken into consideration when regarding different occupational aspects in each subgroup (101).


To determine a possible role of testosterone in status ranking, chess players were tested before and after competition. Before and after tournaments, winners/ winners-to-be had higher testosterone levels than losers (102). Similar results in regard to postcompetition levels were described during tennis matches and wrestling competitions (103, 104). Also a history of previous success or membership in the host team can lead to an elevation of testosterone levels after the match (105). This indicates that personal experience of success as well as the feeling of dominance owing to ‘territorial advantage’ can influence the pattern of endocrine response to a competitive situation. An interdependence with other factors such as aggression and ‘anti-social’ behaviour, described as low levels of intraception, nurturance and responsibility, has been demonstrated (43). Socioeconomic status has been described as a moderating variable in testosterone- behaviour relationships. The constellation of dominance, competitiveness and sensation-seeking behaviour was found to influence the correlation of high testosterone levels with either ‘anti-social’ or ‘prosocial’ behaviour, the latter being dominant in individuals with higher socioeconomic status (106). From these data, a sequence of events was inferred: high testosterone levels could lead to aggressive, ‘antisocial’ behaviour, causing low education levels and thus low occupational status (107). Such a sociological analysis resulting in a singular causal chain is questionable, since hormonal feedback mechanisms and external factors, such as environmental, social and genetic influences should not be disregarded.


Ethnic differences


In violent sexual offenders, serum testosterone levels were significantly higher in the group of native Americans than in Caucasians (41, 108). Such analyses have led to conclusions that a predisposition to crime may be genetically based, have consistent racial variations and may be expressed as serum testosterone levels and brain size, which are supposed to cause different crime rates in Asian, Caucasian and African-Americans (109).


Not biased by selection of subjects in conflict with the law, other studies demonstrate similar testosterone levels in African-American and Caucasian American men. (110, 111). In contradiction to the so-called predisposition theories on crime rates and race, another study involving 1127 men and adjusting data for age and body mass index (BMI), showed levels of total and bioavailable testosterone to be highest in Asian Americans, followed by African-Americans and lowest in Caucasian Americans (112). A greater risk for African-Americans of developing prostate cancer in comparison to Caucasians or Asians seems to exist; testosterone levels are not different, but hereditary factors concerning polymorphism in androgen receptors and 5α-reductase activity are held responsible (113). Supporting results were found comparing Americans of Chinese or Caucasian origin, explaining the lower expression of beard and body hair in Asians (114). Ethnic differences in sex hormone-dependent features are obviously restricted to genetic polymorph-isms, leading to differential enzyme and receptor activity, but not to different testosterone levels (115, 116). The cultural background can also influence testosterone levels through nutritional factors (see Dietary influences; 117).


Sexuality


Androgens play an important role in male sexuality. This is impressively demonstrated by activation/reacti- vation of sexual activity in hypogonadal men receiving substitution therapy. Self-reported decline of sexual functions starts at levels < 5-8 nmol/l (118-120).


Behaviour in male heterosexuality


In eugonadal men, testosterone levels have no significant association with different kinds of sexual activity, except for frequency of masturbation (48). Similar results were reported in couples; in men there was no correlation of testosterone levels to sexual activity of any kind but in women sexual gratification, as well as frequency of intercourse, were positively related to testosterone levels (121). It can be assumed that in men testosterone levels are positively linked to libido, but that sexual activity in the partnership is moderated by the relationship itself. In eugonadal men, external administration of high doses of testosterone was said to increase sexual awareness and arousability, but this is not reflected in any general modification of sexual activity (122, 123).


As seen with other behavioural aspects, a mutual relationship between testosterone levels and specific behaviour (in this case sexuality) can be discerned; a sexual stimulus can increase testosterone levels. This has been demonstrated in male volunteers exposed to films with sexual content (51, 124). Cerebral neuroimaging during such films showed sexually stimulated activation of temporal areas to be associated with testosterone levels. This may reflect a positive feedback effect, as testosterone is released by sexual stimulation and can lead to further activation of these areas (125). These data are consistent not only with natural phenomena observed during sexual arousal, but also with the mutual character of hormone-behaviour interaction.


Behaviour in male homosexuality


It has been suggested that testosterone levels might be decreased in homosexual men. Most of the earlier studies did not find significant differences in total serum testosterone levels but tendencies towards lower free testosterone and higher gonadotropins in comparison to heterosexual men were described (126-128).


A differential organisational effect of possibly sub-normal testosterone levels during foetal brain develop-ment in homosexual men has been discussed. Thus, differences in LH secretion to an oestrogen stimulus, a dissimilar gonadotropin-releasing hormone pulse fre-quency and lower neuronal density of hypothalamic nuclei involved in sexual activity, which have been reported in homosexual men, could be explained (129135). However, homosexuality is as varied and multifold in its behavioural aspects as heterosexuality and postnatal hormone levels do not seem to play a different role from that in heterosexuality.



Violent sexual offences


When sexual offenders receive anti-androgenic gestagens, a clearly hypogonadal state is achieved and sexual activity decreases and, with it, criminal potential. The rate of re-offence is much higher in delinquents not receiving, or discharged from, medication (136). In violent sexual offenders, testosterone serves as a catalyst to initiate the expression of aberrant desires already present, and withdrawal of androgens can inhibit delinquency. The conclusion that higher testosterone levels within or above the normal range would therefore mean a higher risk for sexual aggression cannot be drawn from this information.


Comparing testosterone levels between violent rapists with non-violent sexual offenders and controls has yielded contradictory results (137-139). Sexual aggression as expressed in viewing time of slides with sexually aggressive content vs. control slides showed a slightly positive correlation with free testosterone serum levels (140).


Case reports of patients with low testosterone levels and aggressive sexual behaviour towards women have been published (141-143). Testosterone substitution of these patients who present with often clearly hypo- gonadal androgen levels can cause aggression to disappear (144). Adaption to psychosexual behaviour that is considered normal and cessation of feelings of inferiority evolve during testosterone substitution (145, 146). This observation stresses the importance of environmental and social factors on sexually aberrant behaviour.


Advancing age


‘Andropause’ is a term appearing recently in various publications. Alluding to the female menopause, it indicates that in men there could be a precipitous and definite end to gonadal steroid production and fertility, as it occurs in women. In healthy men, however, the decrease in androgen production is rather a slow process. It was shown that with advancing age the proportion of men presenting with testosterone levels in the sub-normal range increases significantly. Low levels can be found in < 1% of men aged below 40 years, but in >20% of men older than 60 years and > 40% in men above 80 years. These results are confirmed by longitudinal studies. Inter-individual variation is marked and men aged 70 years can still be fertile (147-154). Increased levels of sex hormone binding globulin (SHBG) further decrease bioavailable testosterone. Reduced levels of growth hormone and insulin-like growth factor 1, which inhibit SHBG production in hepatocytes, are responsible for this phenomenon (152). A low protein diet may elevate SHBG levels in elderly men (155). Although healthy older men are more likely to present with lower testosterone levels than younger men, many men aged over 80 years (about 55%) still have relatively normal androgen values. With chronic diseases in different subpopulations, other pictures emerge (see below).


Weight


The BMI, defined as kg/m2, is a useful tool to measure obesity. For the whole range of BMI values there is a negative correlation to serum testosterone levels, but levels of SHBG are also decreased. Thus, levels of bioavailable testosterone remain almost unchanged (156, 157); only in extreme obesity (BMI > 40 kg/m2) do testosterone levels decrease to a greater extent than SHBG levels.


Leptin as a messenger of the body’s energy resources is positively related to BMI; it is inversely correlated to serum testosterone levels (158). Leptin is most probably involved in the regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal (HPG) axis. Although there is a negative correlation between leptin and serum testosterone levels under normal nutritional conditions, another picture emerges in hunger and massively decreased weight; in severe cases, these conditions can affect the HPG axis. Gonadotropins and steroids are reduced, often to clearly hypogonadal levels. Reduced leptin levels and the interaction with neuropeptide Y are held responsible for this phenomenon. In fasting rhesus monkeys, external leptin can restore the function of this axis via normalising (increasing) LH pulse frequency (159).


Dietary influences


The effects on hormone levels of fibre-rich, vegetarian diets and ‘western-type’ diets containing meat protein and more fat have been described. Urinary secretion of androgens (representing a fraction of bioavailable testosterone) in black and white North American men and rural black South African men was found to be significantly higher in those subjects living on a meat- rich western diet. Swapping nutritional habits had corresponding effects (117). Comparing total serum testosterone levels in omnivorous men to those on a vegetarian diet showed no differences, but in the latter group there were significantly higher levels of SHBG, leading to decreased levels of bioavailable testosterone (160). The kind of diet is most likely to influence the fraction of bioavailable testosterone, but not total levels of this hormone (161-165).


Influence of altitude/air pressure


A comparison between boys living at 3600 m or 420 m showed significantly lower levels of saliva testosterone in the first group (100). Corresponding results were reported from men changing the altitude of their place of residence ((166); but also see (11, 167, 168)). Considering the pattern of hormonal change, exposure to low air pressure could mean a type of physical stress that has similar effects as endurance training.


Voice


Mutation of the male voice to a lower frequency occurs during puberty. It is well established that castrates, when orchiectomized before sexual maturation, maintain the high voice of a child. During puberty, there is a clear association between the decreasing frequency of the voice, testicular volume and testosterone levels (169, 170). This correlation can also be found in adult males. In male singers, the deeper voices of bass and baritone singers are related to higher plasma testosterone levels in comparison to tenors. As the deepness of the voice depends on the size of the larynx, it reflects the hormonal situation and organ sensitivity during sexual maturation. That testosterone secretion can be preserved at its relative level is shown in groups of singers 50-70 years of age (171). Due to marked interindividual variation, estimation of testosterone levels by vocal register is not possible in individual persons.



Hair pattern


In contrast to hypogonadal men, many men within the eugonadal range tend to develop balding patterns of the scalp whereas beard growth is not inhibited. Hair growth in children shows that display of so-called terminal hair is not androgen-dependent. In puberty, pubic and axillary hair develop, these follicles being dependent obviously on sexual hormones. The amount of body hair is highly variable and differs between races and families. The degree of male balding is also inherited (172). Density and pattern of distribution of androgen receptors as well as 5α-DHT levels and 5α- reductase activity also play a pivotal role in secondary sex characteristics (173-175). Men without any sign of balding and no beard growth are most likely to be androgen-deficient or -insensitive.


In eugonadal men, a significant association of free testosterone levels with the development of vertex and frontal baldness was reported (176). In another group, healthy young men showed a disposition to balding in those subjects with an increased ratio of 5 a-DHT/ testosterone. Absolute serum testosterone levels were not associated with balding patterns (177). Differences in beard growth, body hair and balding seen between races are probably determined by various degrees of 5α- reductase activity; the ratio 5α-DHT:testosterone was higher in Caucasians than in Asians living in the US and Canada (112).


Influence of systemic non-gonadal diseases on testosterone levels


Major burns, myocardial or cerebral infarctions or traumatic injuries as severe, acutely life-threatening diseases cause extreme stress to the individual. Pulsatile LH secretion is often (reversibly) diminished in these patients, leading clinically to hypogonadal states (178). Gonadal function is also influenced by inflammatory mediators (179). Uraemic hypogonadism can be caused by chronic renal diseases via the pituitary (180). Subnormal testosterone levels but gonadotrophin levels within the eugonadal range can be present in chronic liver diseases (181). Gastrointestinal diseases resulting in maldigestion or malabsorption and, therefore, undernutrition can also have a suppressive effect on testosterone secretion (182). Hyperthyroidism, haematological and autoimmune diseases can also alter androgen production (183, 184).


Influence of medications on testosterone levels


There are multiple pathways by which medications can influence the secretion or the action of androgens. Opiates can alter LH pulsatility; steroidogenic enzymes can be inhibited by ketoconazole. Androgen receptors can be blocked by cimetidine or spironolactone. Testosterone metabolism can be increased via hepatic enzyme induction by barbiturates and other anticonvulsants (183). The use of medication can, especially in uncontrolled, cross-sectional studies, contribute to a bias distorting the results concerning endocrine secretion patterns.


Influence of recreational drugs on testosterone levels


In large study groups, tobacco smoking has been associated with elevated testosterone levels (185, 186). The effect of marijuana smoking has no significant effect on gonadotropin or testosterone levels (187). Excessive alcohol consumption has toxic effects on hepatocytes and leads to hypothalamic dysfunction associated with lower testosterone levels. Moderate consumption of alcohol will have no effect, but a personality trait of sensation-seeking behaviour has been described as associated with higher testosterone levels and higher frequency of alcohol consumption (188). Chronic abuse of cocaine will lead to an impairment of hypothalamic-pituitary function and further to decreased testosterone levels (189). The same applies to chronic abuse of opioids, for example, heroin (190). In the cases of long-term illegal drug abuse or excessive consumption of legal substances, other factors, such as an impaired nutritional state and adverse socioeconomic circumstances, will also influence the endocrine pattern.


Discussion


Many of the studies reviewed here have a controlled design; some, in the case of hormone administration, involve placebo groups. Most of the cross-sectional studies have examined data from larger numbers of participants. However, methodological problems of measuring testosterone in body fluids are often overlooked. Serum testosterone levels are determined routinely by radioimmunoassays or luminescence immunoassays. Because there is a strong diurnal variation with morning concentrations being approximately 20-30% higher than evening values (Fig. 1; (191-197)), samples should always be taken during the morning hours. Only then will single point samples be representative (198-200). Seasonal variations of testosterone levels should also be taken into consideration, at least in studies comparing several groups at different time points (Fig. 1; (201-203)). Because testosterone is a small molecule and present in relatively low concentrations, it is not surprising that there is marked inter- and intra-assay bias and variation (Fig. 2). Therefore, comparing testosterone levels determined with different assays and/or in different laboratories becomes questionable. Quality control uncovers this problem, but has not contributed to its solution. Direct measuring of free testosterone concentrations, which are found at much lower concentrations than the total amount of this steroid (about 2%), is subject to even greater difficulties. The same applies to determination of testosterone levels from saliva samples, which are correlated with free testosterone in serum. Reliable assays are still under development (204). Additional measurement of SHBG and its ratio to total testosterone is more reliable when estimating the bioavailable fraction of testosterone (198, 205). In summary, although assays have been available for over 30 years, measurement of serum testosterone is still relatively unreliable and represents a significant bias when interpreting results and correlating them with biological phenomena. Unfortunately, peer-reviewed journals as the ultimate filter have not contributed much to improve the reliability of published values. Therefore, all reported variations of testosterone levels and their associations with physical and mental aspects must be viewed very critically.


Figure 1. Pattern of relative changes in serum testosterone levels due to diurnal (solid line) and seasonal (dotted line) variations of testosterone secretion (derived from (191-197, 201-203)).


Figure 2. Bias and variation of different testosterone assays in males, derived from a quality control programme on steroid assays with 131 participants (222). This means, e.g. when a sample with a ‘true’ value of total serum testosterone of 12 nmol/l is measured in a laboratory with bias of 15% and variation of 15%, values between 11.7 and 15.9 nmol/l are returned. In another laboratory with bias -15% and variation 15% the same sample is measured between 8.7 to 11.7 nmol/l. Thus, for the ‘true’ value of 12 nmol/l all values between 8.7 and 15.9 nmol were still acceptable for this quality control programme. Open circles, non-isotopic methods; solid squares, RIA; acceptable range marked by full lines.


When interpreting testosterone levels, it is often overlooked that levels of the hormone in body fluids are only a small and transitory step in the cascade of hormone action from production to biological effect. The Leydig cell as the primary source of androgens in the male is subject to influence by various substances such as testosterone itself (206), oestradiol (207) and glucocorticoids, which are also produced in a diurnal rhythm (208). Transforming growth factor-^, basic fibroblast growth factor and tumour necrosis factor-a are other substances that vary with extratesticular changes (i.e. as inflammation markers) and can decrease Leydig cell steroidogenic responsiveness (209-211). Androgen receptor polymorphisms (e.g. CAG repeats) can modify testosterone action. The length of the CAG repeats varies among individuals and these polymorphisms are believed to be related to the transcriptional activity of the activated receptor. This applies to forms of prostate cancer (212), breast cancer (213) and benign prostate hyperplasia (214, 215). It can be assumed that CAG repeat length influences androgen susceptibility in other tissues as well. Tissue-specific androgenicity is not solely dependent on testosterone levels; modification is exerted by tissue-specific coactivators of the androgen receptor (216-220). Finally, gene transcription initiated by the activated androgen receptors can be mediated by polymorphisms in the respective promoter regions (221). Thus, serum testosterone levels are only one of many factors forming the cascade of androgen action and all the other factors may follow a pattern independent of serum testosterone levels.


In the studies discussed here, data have been obtained within a confined scope of parameters (Table 1). In most cases, the drastic effects of testosterone substitution in hypogonadal men are mirrored in eugonadal men by somewhat weaker correlations of endogenous testosterone levels with various aspects. Because of the great inter-individual questionable. A generalisation towards phenomenological structures would also require sublimation of various one-dimensional findings. Combining results concerning hormone interactions with physical or psychological phenomena to create new constructs explaining sociological observations can be dangerous and misleading. It is too easy a step from assigning high testosterone levels to aggressive people to the explanation of legal offences of violent nature and further, discrimination of racial or subcultural groups. This could mean, for example, that persons involved in regular marathon training are likely to show a submissive kind of conflict management and will not develop a balding hair pattern, or that watching blue movies would lead to premature balding which could then be prevented by a strict vegetarian diet or by moving to the mountains. It could mean that a man exposed to increased mental stress is likely to show extraordinary verbal fluency.


Table 1. Testosterone levels: absurdities and prejudices created by applying various one-dimensional associations to individuals. An overview of study results examining singular qualities. Positive associations of testosterone levels of physical and behavioural characteristics.


Regarding all results mentioned, a person with high testosterone levels would then be living at sea level, would refrain from endurance training, enjoy high socioeconomic status as well as daily consumption of meat and sexual pleasures, but avoid gaining too much weight. He would be characterised by increased spatial cognition, but not be able to utter sentences fluently. He growth would be prolific and his head would be bald. He would react rather aggressively to threatening situations. A person with low testosterone levels would be a mountaineer, exercising daily by long runs, living a frugal life with vegetarian food, without a partner and constantly harassed by his environment. He would have a full head of hair, a high-pitched voice, and would converse fluently about his submissive behaviour and his lack of ability to solve mathematical problems (see Table 1).


The described persons might exist, but they are likely to be rare, and just how high their actual testosterone levels would really be remains open to question. The rest of the male population cannot be judged by testosterone levels and we cannot guess their androgen levels by looking at their lives.


Simplifying approaches are likely to produce results with simple structures, which, while logically consistent in themselves, do not conform to the external structures we experience. Many other factors of influence are omitted in these approaches such as experiences that are partially reinforced by hormonal feedback on behaviour that caused them and that social and physical environment and genetic background affect not only an individual’s personality but also a single parameter such as testosterone levels.


Hence, levels of this hormone are influenced by conditions which are partly due to control by the hormone itself, but are also affected by conditions which are beyond individual or hormonal control (Fig. 3). Scientific evaluation of androgen action by carefully planned studies which correct for parameters of additional influence has resulted in useful and fascinating information about the interactions between testosterone and, for example behavioural aspects. It should be viewed for what it is: restricted, but valuable, information on certain domains of hormones and their actions, but it should not be manipulated to create constructs about individuals or groups which inevitably fail. A holistic approach allows a careful application of these principles in individual cases but requires various perspectives of the complex picture of psycho-endocrinological and sociological interactions.


Figure 3. Interactions and feedback-mechanisms of testosterone levels with behavioural and physical dimensions. Arrows indicate the direction of influence, plus or minus signs imply the nature of impact. GH, growth hormone; IGF-1, insulin-like growth factor type 1; DHT, dihydrostestosterone.



Acknowledgements


The authors would like to thank S Nieschlag M.A. for language editing.





References


Sullivan A. The He hormone. The New York Times Magazine 2000, April 2. 

Hackney AC, Fahrner CL & Gulledge TP. Basal reproductive hormonal profiles are altered in endurance trained men. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness 1998 38 138-141. 

Duclos M, Corcuff JB, Rashedi M, Fougere V & Manier G. Does functional alteration of the gonadotropic axis occur in endurance trained athletes during and after exercise? A preliminary study. European Journal of Applied Physiology 1996 73 427-433. 

De Souza MJ, Arce JC, Pescatello LS, Scherzer HS & Luciano AA. Gonadal hormones and semen quality in male runners. A volume threshold effect of endurance training. International Journal of Sports Medicine 1994 15 383-391. 

Hackney AC, Sinning WE & Bruot BC. Hypothalamic-pituitary- testicular axis function in endurance-trained males. International Journal of Sports Medicine 1990 11 298-303. 

Wheeler GD, Wall SR, Belcastro AN & Cumming DC. Reduced serum testosterone and prolactin levels in male distance runners. Journal of the American Medical Association 1984 27 514-516. 

Berchtold P, Berger M, Cuppers HJ, Herrmann J, Nieschlag E, Rudorff K et al. Non-glucoregulatory hormones (T4, T3, rT3, TSH, testosterone) during physical exercise in juvenile type diabetics. Hormone and Metabolic Research 1978 10 269-273. 

Fournier PE, Stalder J, Mermillod B & Chantraine A. Effects of a 110 kilometers ultra-marathon race on plasma hormone levels. International Journal of Sports Medicine 1997 18 252-256. 

Schiirmeyer T, Jung K & Nieschlag E. The effect of an 1100 km run on testicular adrenal and thyroid hormones. International Journal of Andrology 1984 7 276-282. 

Chicharro JL, Lopez-Mojares LM, Lucia A, Perez M, Alvarez J, Labanda P et al. Overtraining parameters in special military units. Aviation Space and Environmental Medicine 1998 69 562568. 

Marinelli M, Roi GS, Giacometti M, Bonini P & Banfi G. Cortisol testosterone and free testosterone in athletes performing a marathon at 4000 m altitude. Hormone Research 1994 41 225-229. 

Vervoorn C, Quist AM, Vermulst LJ, Erich WB, de Vries WR & Thijssen JH. The behaviour of the plasma free testosterone/ cortisol ratio during a season of elite rowing training. International Journal of Sports Medicine 1991 12 25 7-263. 

Singh A, Petrides JS, Gold PW, Chrousos GP & Deuster PA. Differential hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis reactivity to psychological and physical stress. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1999 84 1944-1948. 

Opstad PK. The hypothalamo-pituitary regulation of androgen secretion in young men after prolonged physical stress combined with energy and sleep deprivation. Acta Endocrinol (Copenhagen) 1992 127 231-236. 

Bernton E, Hoover D, Galloway R & Popp K. Adaptation to chronic stress in military trainees. Adrenal androgens, testosterone, glucocorticoids, IGF-1 and immune function. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1995 29 217-231. 

Baumgartner RN, Waters DL, Gallagher D, Morley JE & Garry PJ. Predictors of skeletal muscle mass in elderly men and women. Mechanisms of Ageing and Development 1999 107 123-136. 

Ramos E, Frontera WR, Llopart A & Feliciano D. Muscle strength and hormonal levels in adolescents: gender related differences. International Journal of Sports Medicine 1998 19 526-531. 

Kraemer WJ, Staron RS, Hagerman FC, Hikida RS, Fry AC, Gordon SE et al. The effects of short-term resistance training on endocrine function in men and women. European Journal of Applied Physiology 1998 78 69-76. 

Hakkinen K, Pakarinen A, Newton RU & Kraemer WJ. Acute hormone responses to heavy resistance lower and upper extremity exercise in young versus old men. European Journal of Applied Physiology 1998 77 312-319. 

Kraemer RR, Kilgore JL, Kraemer GR & Castracane VD. Growth hormone IGF-I and testosterone responses to resistive exercise. Medicine andScience inSports andExercise 1992 24 1346-1352. 

Jurimae T, Karelson K, Smirnova T & Viru A. The effect of a single-circuit weight-training session on the blood biochemistry of untrained university students. European Journal of Applied Physiology 1990 61 344-348. 

Bhasin S, Storer TW, Berman N, Callegari C, Clevenger B & Phillips J. The effects of supraphysiologic doses of testosterone on muscle size and strength in normal men. New England Journal of Medicine 1996 335 1-7. 

Craig BW, Brown R & Everhart J. Effects of progressive resistance training on growth hormone and testosterone levels in young and elderly subjects. Mechanisms of Ageing and Development 1989 49 159-169. 

Fry AC, Kraemer WJ & Ramsey LT. Pituitary-adrenal-gonadal responses to high-intensity resistance exercise overtraining. Journal of Applied Physiology 1998 85 2352-2359. 

Bhasin S, Bross R, Storer TW & Casaburi R. Androgens and muscles. In Testosterone: Action Deficiency Substitution, edn. 2, ch 7, pp 209-228. Eds E Nieschlag & HM Behre. Berlin: Springer, 1998. 

Bhasin S, Storer TW Berman N, Yarasheski KE, Clevenger B, Phillips J et al. Testosterone replacement increases fat-free mass and muscle size in hypogonadal men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1997 82 407-413. 

Wang C, Eyre DR, Clark R, Kleinberg D, Newman C, Iranmanesh A et al. Sublingual testosterone replacement improves muscle mass and strength, decreases bone resorption and increases bone formation markers in hypogonadal men-a clinical research center study. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1996 81 3654-3662. 

Snyder PJ, Peachey H, Hannoush P, Berlin JA, Loh L, Lenrow DA et al. Effect of testosterone treatment on body composition and muscle strength in men over 65 years of age. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1999 84 2647-2653. 

Clague JE, Wu FC & Horan MA. Difficulties in measuring the effect of testosterone replacement therapy on muscle function in older men. International Journal of Andrology 1999 22 261-265. 

Singer F & Zumoff B. Subnormal serum testosterone levels in male internal medicine residents. Steroids 1992 57 86-89. 

Chatterton RT Jr, Vogelsong KM, Lu YC & Hudgens GA. Hormonal responses to psychological stress in men preparing for skydiving. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1997 82 2503-2509. 

Chatterton RT Jr & Dooley SL. Reversal of diurnal cortisol rhythm and suppression of plasma testosterone in obstetric residents on call. Journal of the Society for Gynecological Investigation 1999 6 50-54. 

Tegelman R, Carlstrom K & Pousette A. Hormone levels in male ice hockey players during a 26-h cup tournament. International Journal of Andrology 1988 11 361-368. 

Schulz P Walker JP Peyrin L, Soulier V Curtin F & Steimer T. Lower sex hormones in men during anticipatory stress. Neuroreport 1996 25 3101-3104. 

Francis KT. The relationship between high and low trait psychological stress, serum testosterone and serum cortisol. Experientia 1981 37 1296-1297. 

Nilsson PM, Moller L & Solstad K. Adverse effects of psychosocial stress on gonadal function and insulin levels in middle-aged males. Journal of Internal Medicine 1995 237 479-486. 

MacLean CR, Walton KG, Wenneberg SR, Levitsky DK, Mandarino JP, Waziri R et al. Effects of the transcendental meditation program on adaptive mechanisms: changes in hormone levels and responses to stress after 4 months of practice. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1997 22 277-295. 

Grossi G, Theorell T, Jurisoo M & Setterlind S. Psychophysiological correlates of organizational change and threat of unemployment among police inspectors. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science 1999 34 30-42. 

Hardy MP & Ganjam VK. Stress, 11beta-HSD and Leydig cell function. Journal of Andrology 1997 18 475-479. 

Rejeski WJ, Brubaker PH, Herb RA, Kaplan JR & Koritnik DJ. Anabolic steroids and aggressive behaviour in cynomolgus monkeys. Behavioral Medicine 1988 11 95-105. 

Brooks JH & Reddon JR. Serum testosterone in violent and nonviolent young offenders. Journal of Clinical Psychology 1996 52 475-483. 

Kreuz LE & Rose RM. Assessment of aggressive behaviour and plasma testosterone in a young criminal population. Psychosomatic Medicine 1972 34 321-332. 

Ehrenkranz J, Bliss E & Sheard MH. Plasma testosterone correlation with aggressive behaviour and social dominance in man. Psychosomatic Medicine 1974 36 469-474. 

Virkkunen M, Kallio E, Rawlings R, Tokola R, Poland RE, Guidotti A et al. Personality profiles and state aggressiveness in Finnish alcoholic violent offenders fire setters and healthy volunteers. Archives of General Psychiatry 1994 51 28-33. 

Bergman B & Brismar B. Characteristics of imprisoned wife- beaters. Forensic Science International 1994 13 157-167. 

Banks T & Dabbs JM Jr. Salivary testosterone and cortisol in a delinquent and violent urban subculture. Journal of the Society of Psychology 1996 136 49-56. 

Aromaki AS, Lindman RE & Eriksson CJP. Testosterone aggressiveness and antisocial personality. Aggressive Behavior 1999 25 113-123. 

Monti PM, Brown WA & Corriveau DP. Testosterone and components of aggressive and sexual behaviour in man. American Journal of Psychiatry 1977 134 692-694. 

Scaramella TJ & Brown WA. Serum testosterone and aggressiveness in hockey players. Psychosomatic Medicine 1978 40 262-265. 

Cohen D, Nisbett RE, Bowdle BF & Schwarz N. Insult aggression and the southern culture of honor: an ‘experimental ethnography’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1996 70 945-959. 

Hellhammer DH, Hubert W & Schiirmeyer T. Changes in saliva testosterone after psychological stimulation in men. Psycho-neuroendocrinology 1985 10 77-81. 

Archer J. The influence of testosterone on human aggression. British Journal of Psychology 1991 82 1-28. 

Adler L, Wedekind D, Pilz J, Weniger G & Huether G. Endocrine correlates of personality traits: a comparison between emotionally stable and emotionally labile healthy young men. Neuropsychobiology 1997 35 205-210. 

Pope HG Jr, Kouri EM & Hudson JI. Effects of supraphysiologic doses of testosterone on mood and aggression in normal men: a randomized controlled trial. Archives of General Psychiatry 2000 57 133-140. 

Christiansen K. Behavioural correlates of testosterone. In Testosterone: Action Deficiency Substitution, edn. 2, ch 3, pp 107-142. Eds E Nieschlag & HM Behre. Berlin: Springer, 1998. 

Kouri EM, Lukas SE, Pope HG Jr & Oliva PS. Increased aggressive responding in male volunteers following the administration of gradually increasing doses of testosterone cypionate. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 1995 40 73-79. 

Tricker R, Casaburi R, Storer TW, Clevenger B, Berman N, Shirazi A et al. The effects of supraphysiological doses of testosterone on angry behaviour in healthy eugonadal men-a clinical research center study. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1996 81 3754-3758. 

Nieschlag E. Testosteron Anabolika und aggressives Verhalten bei Männern. Deutsches Arzteblatt 1992 89 2967-2972. 

Burris AS, Banks SM, Carter CS, Davidson JM & Sherins RJ. A long-term prospective study of the physiologic and behavioural effects of hormone replacement in untreated hypogonadal men. Journal of Andrology 1992 13 297-304. 

Schweiger U, Deuschle M, Weber B, Korner A, Lammers CH, Schmider J et al. Testosterone gonadotropin and cortisol secretion in male patients with major depression. Psychosomatic Medicine 1999 61 292-296. 

Unden F, Ljunggren JG, Beck-Friis J, Kjellman BF & Wetterberg L. Hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis in major depressive disorders. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 1988 78 138-146. 

Yehuda R, Teicher MH, Trestman RL, Levengood RA & Siever LJ. Cortisol regulation in posttraumatic stress disorder and major depression: a chronobiological analysis. Biological Psychiatry 1996 15 79-88. 

Maes M, Lin A, Bonaccorso S, van Hunsel F, Van Gastel A, Delmeire L et al. Increased 24-h urinary cortisol excretion in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder and patients with major depression but not in patients with fibromyalgia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 1998 98 328-335. 

Vogel W, Klaiber EL & Brovermann DM. Roles of the gonadal steroid hormones in psychiatric depression in men and women. Progressions in Neurology and Psychopharmacology 1978 2 487 503. 

Seidman SN & Rabkin JG. Testosterone replacement therapy for hypogonadal men with SSRI-refractory depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 1998 48 157-161. 

Hyde JS, Fennema E & Lamon SJ. Gender differences in mathematics performance: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 1990 107 139-155. 

McGlone J. Sex differences in human brain organization: a critical survey. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1980 3 215-227. 

Hassler M, Gupta D & Wollmann H. Testosterone, estradiol, ACTH and musical, spatial and verbal performance. International Journal of Neuroscience 1992 65 45-60. 

Voyer D, Voyer S & Bryden MP. Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin 1995 117 250-270. 

Karadi K, Szabo I, Szepesi T, Kallai J & Kovacs B. Sex differences on the hand mental rotation task for 9-yr-old children and young adults. Perceptual and Motor Skills 1999 89 969-972. 

Crucian GP & Berenbaum SA. Sex differences in right hemisphere tasks. Brain and Cognition 1998 36 377-389. 

Collins DW & Kimura D. A large sex difference on a twodimensional mental rotation task. Behavioral Neuroscience 1997 111 845-849. 

Neave N, Menaged M & Weightman DR. Sex differences in cognition: the role of testosterone and sexual orientation. Brain and Cognition 1999 41 245-262. 

Levy J & Reid M. Variations in cerebral organization as a function of handedness, hand posture in writing, and sex. JournalofExperimentalPsychology: General 1978 107 119-144. 

Harris IM, Egan GF, Sonkkila C, Tochon-Danguy HJ, Paxinos G & Watson JD. Selective right parietal lobe activation during mental rotation: a parametric PETstudy. Brain 2000 123 65-73. 

Alivisatos B & Petrides M. Functional activation of the human brain during mental rotation. Neuropsychologia 1997 35 111 118. 

Silverman I, Kastuk D, Choi J & Phillips K. Testosterone levels and spatial ability in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1999 24 813-822. 

Christiansen K & Knussmann R. Sex hormones and cognitive functioning in men. Neuropsychobiology 1987 18 27-36. 

Moffat SD & Hampson E. A curvilinear relationship between testosterone and spatial cognition in humans: possible influence of hand preference. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1996 21 323-337. 

Gouchie C & Kimura D. The relationship between testosterone levels and cognitive ability patterns. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1991 16 323-334. 

Kimura D & Hampson E. Cognitive pattern in men and women is influenced by fluctuations in sex hormones. Current Opinions in Psychological Sciences 1994 3 57-61. 

Gordon HW, Stoffer DS & Lee PA. Ultradian rhythms in performance on tests of specialized cognitive functions. International Journal of Neuroscience 1995 83 199-211. 

Slabbekoorn D, van Goozen SH, Megens J, Gooren LJ & Cohen- Kettenis PT. Activating effects of cross-sex hormones on cognitive functioning: a study of short-term and long-term hormone effects in transsexuals. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1999 24 423-447. 

Imperato-McGinley J, Pichardo M, Gautier T, Voyer D & Bryden MP. Cognitive abilities in androgen-insensitive subjects: comparison with control males and females from the same kindred. Clinical Endocrinology 1991 34 341-347. 

Alexander GM, Swerdloff RS, Wang C, Davidson T, McDonald V, Steiner B et al Androgen-behaviour correlations in hypo- gonadal men and eugonadal men II Cognitive abilities. Hormones and Behavior 1998 33 85-94. 

Buchsbaum MS & Henkin RI. Perceptual abnormalities in patients with chromatin negative gonadal dysgenesis and hypogonadotropic hypogonadism. International Journal of Neuroscience 1980 11 201-209. 

Hier DB & Crowley WF Jr. Spatial ability in androgen-deficient men. New England Journal of Medicine 1982 306 1202-1205. 

Janowsky JS, Oviat SK & Orwoll ES. Testosterone influences spatial cognition in older men. Behavioral Neuroscience 1994 108 325-332. 

Zitzmann M, Weckesser M, Schober O & Nieschlag E. Testosterone-induced changes in cerebral glucose metabolism: association with visuospatial capability (submitted). 

Wolf OT, Preut R, Hellhammer DH, Kudielka BM, Schurmeyer TH & Kirschbaum C. Testosterone and cognition in elderly men: a single testosterone injection blocks the practice effect in verbal fluency, but has no effect on spatial or verbal memory. Biological Psychiatry 2000 47 650-654. 

Miles C, Green R, Sanders G & Hines M. Estrogen and memory in a transsexual population. Hormones and Behavior 1998 34 199-208. 

Wolf OT, Kudielka BM, Hellhammer DH, Torber S, McEwen BS & Kirschbaum C. Two weeks of transdermal estradiol treatment in postmenopausal elderly women and its effect on memory and mood: verbal memory changes are associated with the treatment induced estradiol levels. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1999 24 727-741. 

Carlson LE & Sherwin BB. Higher levels of plasma estradiol and testosterone in healthy elderly men compared with age-matched 

women may protect aspects of explicit memory. Menopause 2000 7 168-177. 

Resnick SM, Maki PM, Golski S, Kraut MA & Zonderman AB. Effects of estrogen replacement therapy on PET cerebral blood flow and neuropsychological performance. Hormones and Behavior 1998 34 171-182. 

Hassler M. Creative musical behaviour and sex hormones: musical talent and spatial ability in the two sexes. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1992 17 55-70. 

Hassler M & Nieschlag E. Masculinity, femininity and musical composition, psychological and psychoendocrinological aspects of musical and spatial faculties. Archiv fuer Psychologie 1989 141 71-84. 

Wickings EJ & Dixson AF. Testicular function secondary sexual development and social status in male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Physiology and Behavior 1992 52 909-916. 

Winslow JT, Ellingboe J & Miczek KA. Effects of alcohol on aggressive behaviour in squirrel monkeys: influence of testosterone and social context. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1988 95 356-363. 

Schaal B, Tremblay RE, Soussignan R & Susman EJ. Male testosterone linked to high social dominance but low physical aggression in early adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 1996 35 1322-1330. 

Fellmann N, Beaune B & Coudert J. Blood lactate after maximal and supramaximal exercise in 10- to 12-year-old Bolivian boys. Effects of altitude and socioeconomic status. International Journal of Sports Medicine 1994 15 (Suppl 2) S90-S95. 

Theorell T, Ahlberg HG, Sigala F, Perski A, Soderholm M, Kallner A et al. Psychosocial and biomedical comparison between men in six contrasting service occupations. Work Stress 1990 4 51-63. 

Mazur A, Booth A & Dabbs JM Jr. Testosterone and chess competition. Social Psychology Quarterly 1992 55 70-77. 

Mazur A & Lamb TA. Testosterone status and mood in human males. Hormones and Behavior 1980 14 236-246. 

Elias M. Serum cortisol testosterone and testosterone binding globulin responses to competitive fighting human males. Aggressive Behaviour 1981 7 215-224. 

Suay F, Salvador A, Gonzalez-Bono E, Sanchis C, Martinez M, Martinez-Sanchis S et al. Effects of competition and its outcome on serum testosterone cortisol and prolactin. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1999 24 551-566. 

Dabbs JM Jr & Morris R. Testosterone social class and antisocial behaviour in a sample of 4462 men. Psychological Science 1990 1 209-211. 

Dabbs JM Jr. Testosterone and occupational achievement. Social Forces 1992 70 813-824. 

Studer LH, Reddon JR & Siminoski KG. Serum testosterone in adult sex offenders: a comparison between Caucasians and North American Indians. Journal of Clinical Psychology 1997 53 375-385. 

Rushton JP. Race and crime: an international dilemma. Society 1995 32 37-41. 

Zagars GK, Pollack A & Pettaway CA. Prostate cancer in African-American men: outcome following radiation therapy with or without adjuvant androgen ablation. International Journal of Radiation Oncology Biology Physics 1998 42 517-523. 

Gombe S & Ndeti CS. Plasma testosterone and progesterone levels in adult East Africans. East African Medical Journal 1976 53 43-46. 

Wu AH, Whittemore AS, Kolonel LN, John EM, Gallagher RP, West DW et al. Serum androgens and sex hormone-binding globulins in relation to lifestyle factors in older African- American, white and Asian men in the United States and Canada. Cancer Epidemiology Biomarkers and Prevention 1995 4 735-741. 

Cussenot O, Valeri A, Berthon P, Fournier G & Mangin P. Hereditary prostate cancer and other genetic predispositions to prostate cancer. Urology International 60 1998 30-34. 

Hikim AP, Wang C, Lue Y, Johnson L, Wang XH & Swerdloff RS. Spontaneous germ cell apoptosis in humans: evidence for ethnic differences in the susceptibility of germ cells to programmed cell death. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1998 83 152-156. 

Wang Q, Ghadessy FJ & Yong EL. Analysis of the transactivation domain of the androgen receptor in patients with male infertility. Clinical Genetics 1998 54 185-192. 

Johnson L, Barnard JJ, Rodriguez L, Smith EC, Swerdloff RS, Wang XH et al Ethnic differences in testicular structure and spermatogenic potential may predispose testes of Asian men to a heightened sensitivity to steroidal contraceptives. Journal of Andrology 1998 19 348-35 7. 

Hill P, Wynder EL, GarbaczewskiL, Garnes H & Walker AR. Diet and urinary steroids inblack and white North Americanmen and black SouthAfrican men. Cancer Research 1979 39 5101-5105. 

Gooren LJ. Androgen levels and sex functions in testosterone- treated hypogonadal men. Archives of Sexual Behavior 1987 16 463-473. 

Clopper RR, Voorhess ML, MacGillivray MH, Lee PA & Mills B. Psychosexual behaviour in hypopituitary men: a controlled comparison of gonadotropin and testosterone replacement. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1993 18 149-161. 

Bals-Pratsch M, Langer K, Place VA & Nieschlag E. Substitution therapy of hypogonadal men with transdermal testosterone over one year. Acta Endocrinologica 1988 118 7-13. 

Persky H, Lief HI, Strauss D, Miller WR & O’Brien CP. Plasma testosterone level and sexual behaviour of couples. Archives of Sexual Behavior 1978 7 15 7-173. 

Anderson RA, Bancroft J & Wu FC. The effects of exogenous testosterone on sexuality and mood of normal men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1992 75 1503-1507. 

Alexander GM, Swerdloff RS, Wang C, Davidson T, McDonald V, Steiner B et al. Androgen-behaviour correlations in hypogonadal men and eugonadal men I. Mood and response to auditory sexual stimuli. Hormones and Behavior 1997 31 110119. 

Stoleru SG, Ennaji A, Cournot A & Spira A. LH pulsatile secretion and testosterone blood levels are influenced by sexual arousal in human males. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1993 18 205-218. 

Stoleru S, Gregoire MC, Gerard D, Decety J, Lafarge E, Cinotti L et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of visually evoked sexual arousal in human males. Archives of Sexual Behavior 1999 28 1-21. 

James S, Carter RA & Orwin A. Significance of androgen levels in the aetiology and treatment of homosexuality. Psychological Medicine 1977 7 427-429. 

Rohde W, Stahl F & Dorner G. Plasma basal levels of FSH, LH and testosterone in homosexual men. Endokrinologie 1977 70 

241-248. 

Doerr P, Pirke KM, Kockott G & Dittmar F. Further studies on sex hormones in male homosexuals. Archives of General Psychiatry 1976 33 611-614. 

D orner G. Neuroendocrine response to estrogen and brain differentiation in heterosexuals, homosexuals and transsexuals. Archives of Sexual Behavior 1988 17 57-75. 

Elias AN & Valenta LJ. Are all males equal? Anatomic and functional basis for sexual orientation in males. Medical Hypotheses 1992 39 85-87. 

Headings VE. Etiology of homosexuality. Southern Medical Journal 1980 73 1024-1027. 

D orner G, Docke F, Gotz F, Rohde W, Stahl F & Tonjes R. Sexual differentiation of gonadotrophin secretion, sexual orientation and gender role behaviour. Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology 1987 27 1081-1087. 

Giordano G & Giusti M. Hormones and psychosexual differentiation. Minerva Endocrinologica 1995 20 165-193. 

Schmidt G & Clement U. Does peace prevent homosexuality? Journal of Homosexuality 1995 28 269-275. 

Banks A & Gartrell NK. Hormones and sexual orientation: a questionable link. Journal of Homosexuality 1995 28 247-268. 

Meyer WJ 3d, Cole C & Emory E. Depo provera treatment for sex offending behaviour: an evaluation of outcome. Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 1992 20 249-259. 

Rada RT, Laws DR, Kellner R, Stivastava L & Peake G. Plasma androgens in violent and nonviolent sex offenders. Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 1983 11 149-158. 

Bradford JM & McLean D. Sexual offenders violence and testosterone: a clinical study. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 1984 29 335-343. 

Rada RT, Laws DR & Kellner R. Plasma testosterone levels in the rapist. Psychosomatic Medicine 1976 38 257-268. 

Christiansen K & Knussmann R. Androgen levels and components of aggressive behaviour in men. Hormones and Behavior 1987 21 170-180. 

Sourial N & Fenton F. Testosterone treatment of an XXYY male presenting with aggression: a case report. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 1988 33 846-850. 

Bernstein R, Berman IW & Nurse GT. An XXYY male presenting with aggression: a case report. South African Medical Journal 1977 52 809-812. 

Beckmann J, Dupont A, Erling I, Jacobsen P, Mikkelsen M & Theilgaard A. Report of sex chromosome abnormalities in mentally retarded male offenders including a psychological study of patients with XYY and XXYY karyotypes. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research 1974 18 331-353. 

Nielsen J, Pelsen B & Sorensen K. Follow-up of 30 Klinefelter males treated with testosterone. Clinical Genetics 1988 33 262269. 

Seim HC & Dwyer M. Evaluation of serum testosterone and luteinizing hormone levels in sex offenders. Family Practice Research Journal 1988 7 175-180. 

Shostakovich BV, Smirnova LK, Tkachenko AA, Ushakova IM, Kartelishev AV & Nikolaeva TN. A comparative evaluation of the biochemical and psychopathological characteristics in subjects with signs of pedophilia. Zhurnal Nevropatologii I Psikhiatrii Imeni S.S. Korsakova 1992 92 83-88. 

Nieschlag E, Kley HK, Wiegelmann W, Solbach HG & Kriiskemper HL. Age and endocrine function of the testes in adult man. Deutsche Medizinische Wochenschrift 1973 29 12811284. 

Nieschlag E, Lammers U, Freischem CW, Langer K & Wickings EJ. Reproductive functions in young fathers and grandfathers. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1982 55 676681. 

Vermeulen A & Deslypere JP. Testicular endocrine function in the ageing male. Maturitas 1985 7 273-279. 

Schiavi RC, Schreiner-Engel P, White D & Mandeli J. The relationship between pituitary-gonadal function and sexual behaviour in healthy aging men. Psychosomatic Medicine 1991 53 363-374. 

Zitzmann M, Behre HM & Nieschlag E. Choice of testosterone preparation for the management of the aging male. In Endocrine Basis of Reproductive Function, edn 1, pp 551-567. Ed. MFilicori. Monduzzi, Italy, 2000. 

Vermeulen A, Kaufman JM & Giagulli VA. Influence of some biological indexes on sex hormone-binding globulin and androgen levels in aging or obese males. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1996 81 1821-1826. 

Morley JE, Kaiser FE, Perry HM 3rd, Patrick P, Morley PM, Stauber PM et al. Longitudinal changes in testosterone luteinizing hormone and follicle-stimulating hormone in healthy older men. Metabolism 1997 46 410-413. 

Kaufman JM & Vermeulen A. Androgens in male senescence. In Testosterone: Action Deficiency Substitution, edn. 2, ch 16, pp 437-472. Eds E Nieschlag & HM Behre. Berlin: Springer, 1998. 

Longcope C, Feldman HA, McKinlay JB & Araujo AB. Diet and sex hormone-binding globulin. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 2000 85 293-296. 

Giagulli VA, Kaufman JM & Vermeulen A. Pathogenesis of the decreased androgen levels in obese men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1994 79 997-1000. 

Barrett-Connor E, Khaw KT & Yen SS. Endogenous sex hormone levels in older adult men with diabetes mellitus. American Journal of Epidemiology 1990 132 895-901. 

Behre HM, Simoni M & Nieschlag E. Strong association between serum levels of leptin and testosterone in men. Clinical Endocrinology 1997 47 237-240. 

Finn PD, Cunningham MJ, Pau KY, Spies HG, Clifton DK & Steiner RA. The stimulatory effect of leptin on the neuroendocrine reproductive axis of the monkey. Endocrinology 1998 139 4652-4662. 

Belanger A, Locong A, Noel C, Cusan L, Dupont A, Prevost J et al. Influence of diet on plasma steroids and sex hormone-binding globulin levels in adult men. Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology 1989 32 829-833. 

Key TJ, Roe L, Thorogood M, Moore JW, Clark GM & Wang DY. Testosterone sex hormone-binding globulin, calculated free testosterone and oestradiol in male vegans and omnivores. British Journal of Nutrition 1990 64 111-119. 

Bishop DT, Meikle AW, Slattery ML, Stringham JD, Ford MH & West DW. The effect of nutritional factors on sex hormone levels in male twins. Genetic Epidemiology 1988 5 43-59. 

Reed MJ, Cheng RW, Simmonds M, Richmond W & James VH. Dietary lipids: an additional regulator of plasma levels of sex hormone binding globulin. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1987 64 1083-1085. 

Meikle AW, Stringham JD, Woodward MG & McMurry MP Effects of a fat-containing meal on sex hormones in men. Metabolism 1990 39 943-946. 

Haffner SM, Valdez RA, Mykkanen L, Stern MP & Katz MS. Decreased testosterone and dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate concentrations are associated with increased insulin and glucose concentrations in nondiabetic men. Metabolism 1994 43 599-603. 

Sawhney RC, Chabra PC, Malhotra AS, Singh T, Riar SS & Rai RM. Hormone profiles at high altitude in man. Andrologia 1985 17 178-184. 

Basu M, Pal K, Prasad R, Malhotra AS, Rao KS & Sawhney RC. Pituitary gonadal and adrenal hormones after prolonged residence at extreme altitude in man. International Journal of Andrology 1997 20 153-158. 

Ajlouni KM, Sliman NA, Najdawi A, Abu-Hajir MM & Arnaout MA. Effect of exposure to the altitude of 300 meters below sea-level on testosterone, luteinizing hormone, follicle- stimulating hormone and prolactin in man. Journal of Endocrinological Investigation 1988 11 621-623. 

Harries ML, Walker JM, Williams DM, Hawkins S & Hughes IA. Changes in the male voice at puberty. Archives of Disease in Childhood 1997 77 445-447. 

Pedersen MF, Moller S, Krabbe S & Bennett P Fundamental voice frequency measured by electroglottography during continuous speech. A new exact secondary sex characteristic in boys in puberty. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 1986 11 21-27. 

Meuser W & Nieschlag E. Sex hormones and depth of voice in the male. Deutsche Medizinische Wochenschrift 1977 25 261264. 

Randall VA. Androgens and hair. In Testosterone: Action Deficiency Substitution, edn 2, ch 5, pp 169-186. Eds E Nieschlag & HM Behre. Berlin: Springer, 1998. 

Hibberts NA, Howell AE & Randall VA. Balding hair follicle dermal papilla cells contain higher levels of androgen receptors than those from non-balding scalp. Journal of Endocrinology 1998 156 59-65. 

Randall VA, Thornton MJ, Hamada K & Messenger AG. Mechanism of androgen action in cultured dermal papilla cells derived from human hair follicles with varying responses to androgens in vivo. Journal of Investigative Dermatology 98 (6 Suppl) 1992 86S-91S. 

Kaufman KD, Olsen EA, Whiting D, Savin R, DeVillez R, Bergfeld W et al Finasteride in the treatment of men with androgenetic alopecia. Finasteride Male Pattern Hair Loss Study Group. Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology 1998 39 578-589. 

Demark-Wahnefried W, Lesko SM, Conaway MR, Robertson CN, Clark RV Lobaugh B et al. Serum androgens: associations with prostate cancer risk and hair patterning. Journal of Andrology 1997 18 495-500. 

Knussmann R, Christiansen K & Kannmacher J. Relations between sex hormone level and characters of hair and skin in healthy young men. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 1992 88 59-67. 

Dong Q, Hawker F, McWilliam D, Bangah M, Burger H & Handelsman DJ. Circulating immunoreactive inhibin and testosterone levels in men with critical illness. Clinical Endocrinology 1992 36 399-404. 

Handelsman DJ & Staraj S. Testicular size: the effects of aging, malnutrition and illness. Journal of Andrology 1985 6 144-151. 

Handelsman DJ & Dong Q. Hypothalamo-pituitary gonadal axis in chronic renal failure. Endocrinology and Metabolism Clinics of North America 1993 22 145-161. 

Handelsman DJ, Strasser S, McDonald JA, Conway AJ & McCaughan GW. Hypothalamic-pituitary-testicular function in end-stage non-alcoholic liver disease before and after liver transplantation. Clinical Endocrinology 1995 43 331-337. 

Zelissen PM, van Hattum J, Poen H, Scholten P, Gerritse R & te Velde ER. Influence of salazosulphapyridine and 5-amino- salicylic acid on seminal qualities and male sex hormones. Scandinavian Journal of Gastroenterology 1988 23 1100-1104. 

Handelsman DJ. Testicular dysfunction in systemic diseases. In Andrology: Male Reproductive Health and Dysfunction, edn 2, ch 12, pp 227-235. Eds E Nieschlag & HM Behre. Berlin Heidelberg: Springer, 2000. 

Liu PY & Handelsman DJ. Androgen therapy in non-gonadal diseases. In Testosterone: Action Deficiency Substitution, edn. 2, ch 17, pp 473-512. Eds E Nieschlag & HM Behre. Berlin: Springer, 1998. 

Fisher CL, Mannino DM, Herman WH & Frumkin H. Cigarette smoking and thyroid hormone levels in males. International Journal of Epidemiology 1997 26 972-977. 

Knussmann R & Christiansen K. Attributes of masculinity and androgen measures. Homo 1989 39 45-50. 

Cushman P Jr. Plasma testosterone levels in healthy male marijuana smokers. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse 1975 2 269-275. 

La Grange L, Jones TD, Erb L & Reyes E. Alcohol consumption: biochemical and personality correlates in a college student population. Addictive Behaviors 1995 20 93-103. 

Sarnyai Z, Dhabhar FS, McEwen BS & Kreek MJ. Neuroendo-crine-related effects of long-term ‘binge’ cocaine administration: diminished individual differences in stress-induced corticosterone response. Neuroendocrinology 1998 68 334-344. 

Rasheed A & Tareen IA. Effects of heroin on thyroid function, cortisol and testosterone level in addicts. Polish Journal of Pharmacology 1995 47 441-444. 

Simoni M, Montanini V, Fustini MF, Del Rio G, Cioni K & Marrama P. Circadian rhythm of plasma testosterone in men with idiopathic hypogonadotrophic hypogonadism before and during pulsatile administration of gonadotrophin-releasing hormone. Clinical Endocrinology 1992 36 29-34. 

Ellison PT & Panter-Brick C. Salivary testosterone levels among Tamang and Kami males of central Nepal. Human Biology 1996 68 955-965. 

Gritsko N, Shulga V & Ivanova L. Diurnal rhythms of the neuroendocrine system in professional riveters with different constitutional types. Central European Journal of Public Health 3 132-134. 

Nieschlag E & Ismail AA. Diurnal variations of plasma testosterone in normal and pathological conditions as measured by the technique of competitive protein binding. Journal of Endocrinology 1970 46 3-4. 

Behre HM, Klein B, Steinmeyer E, McGregor GP Voigt K & Nieschlag E. Effective suppression of luteinizing hormone and testosterone by single doses of the new gonadotropin-releasing hormone antagonist cetrorelix (SB-75) in normal men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1992 75 393-398. 

Albertsson-Wikland K, Rosberg S, Lannering B, Dunkel L, Selstam G & Norjavaara E. Twenty-four-hour profiles of luteinizing hormone, follicle-stimulating hormone, testosterone, and estradiol levels: a semilongitudinal study throughout puberty in healthy boys. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1997 82 541-549. 

Carlsen E, Olsson C, Petersen JH, Andersson AM & Skakkebaek NE. Diurnal rhythm in serum levels of inhibin B in normal men: relation to testicular steroids and gonadotropins. Journal ofClinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1999 84 1664-1669. 

Nieschlag E. Testicular function. In Clinical Laboratory Diagnostics, ch 11, pp 1100-1108. Ed. LThomas. Frankfurt-Main: TH- Books, 1998. 

Zitzmann M & Nieschlag E. Hormone substitution in male hypogonadism. Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology 2000 161 73-88. 

Vermeulen A & Verdonck G. Representativeness of a single point plasma testosterone level for the long term hormonal milieu in men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1992 74 939-942. 

Meriggiola MC, Noonan EA, Paulsen CA & Bremner WJ. Annual patterns of luteinizing hormone, follicle stimulating hormone testosterone and inhibin in normal men. Human Reproduction 11 248-252. 

Maes M, Mommen K, Hendrickx D, Peeters D, D’Hondt P, Ranjan R et al. Components of biological variation including seasonality in blood concentrations of TSH, TT3, FT4, PRL, cortisol and testosterone in healthy volunteers. Clinical Endocrinology 1997 46 587-598. 

Dabbs JM Jr. Age and seasonal variation in serum testosterone concentration among men. Chronobiology International 1990 7 245-249. 

Tschop M, Behre HM, Nieschlag E, Dressendorfer RA & Strasburger CJ. A time-resolved fluorescence immunoassay for the measurement of testosterone in saliva: monitoring of testosterone replacement therapy with testosterone buciclate. Clinical Chemistry and Laboratory Medicine 1998 36 223-230. 

Vermeulen A, Verdonck L & Kaufman JM. A critical evaluation of simple methods for the estimation of free testosterone in serum. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1999 84 3666-3672. 

Burgos-Trinidad M, Youngblood GL, Maroto MR, Scheller A, Robins DM & Payne AH. Repression of cAMP-induced expression of the mouse P450 17 alpha-hydroxylase/C17-20 lyase gene (Cyp17) by androgens. Molecular Endocrinology 1997 11 87-96. 

Zhai J, Lanclos KD & Abney TO. Estrogen receptor messenger ribonucleic acid changes during Leydig cell development. 

Biology of Reproduction 1996 55 782-788. 

Aguiar BM & Vind C. Effects of dexamethasone on steroidogenesis in Leydig ceils from rats of different ages. Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology 1995 54 75-81. 

Le Roy C, Lejeune H, Chuzel F, Saez JM & Langlois D. Autocrine regulation of Leydig cell differentiated functions by insulin-like growth factor I and transforming growth factor beta. Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology 1999 69 379-384. 

Laslett AL, McFarlane JR & Risbridger GP. Developmental response by Leydig cells to acidic and basic fibroblast growth factor. Journal ofSteroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology 1997 60 171-179. 

Xiong Y & Hales DB. Expression regulation and production of tumor necrosis factor-alpha in mouse testicular interstitial macrophages in vitro. Endocrinology 1993 133 2568-2573. 

Xue W, Irvine RA, Yu MC, Ross RK, Coetzee GA & Ingles SA. Susceptibility to prostate cancer: interaction between genotypes at the androgen receptor and prostate-specific antigen loci. Cancer Research 2000 60 839-841. 

Yu H, Bharaj B, Vassilikos EJ, Giai M & Diamandis EP. Shorter CAG repeat length in the androgen receptor gene is associated with more aggressive forms of breast cancer. Breast Cancer Research and Treatment 2000 59 153-161. 

Mitsumori K, Terai A, Oka H, Segawa T, Ogura K, Yoshida O et al. Androgen receptor CAG repeat length polymorphism in benign prostatic hyperplasia (BPH): correlation with adenoma growth. Prostate 1999 41 253-257. 

Giovannucci E, Platz EA, Stampfer MJ, Chan A, Krithivas K & Kawachi I et al. The CAG repeat within the androgen receptor gene and benign prostatic hyperplasia. Urology 1999 53 121125. 

Needham M, Raines S, McPheat J, Stacey C, Ellston J, Hoare S et al. Differential interaction of steroid hormone receptors with LXXLL motifs in SRC-1a depends on residues flanking the motif. Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology 2000 72 35-46. 

Muller JM, Isele U, Metzger E, Rempel A, Moser M, Pscherer A et al. FHL2 a novel tissue-specific coactivator of the androgen receptor. EMBO Journal 2000 19 359-369. 

Lee DK, Duan HO & Chang C. From androgen receptor to the general transcription factor TFIIH. Identification of cdk activating kinase (CAK) as an androgen receptor NH(2)-terminal associated coactivator. Journal of Biological Chemistry 2000 275 9308-9313. 

Hsiao PW, Lin DL, Nakao R & Chang C. The linkage of Kennedy’s neuron disease to ARA24, the first identified androgen receptor polyglutamine region-associated coactivator. Journal of Biological Chemistry 1999 274 20229-20234. 

Tan JA, Hall SH, Petrusz P & French FS. Thyroid receptor activator molecule TRAM-1, is an androgen receptor coactivator. Endocrinology 2000 141 3440-3450. 

Lu S, Jenster G & Epner DE. Androgen induction of cyclin- dependent kinase inhibitor p21 gene: role of androgen receptor and transcription factor Sp1 complex. Molecular Endocrinology 2000 14 753-760. 

UK NEQAS for steroid hormones, Annual Report 1996.