Olivier Beauchet
Department of Geriatrics, Saint-Etienne University Hospitals, 42055 Saint-Etienne cedex 2, France
O Beauchet; E-mail: olivierbeauchet@univ-st-etienne.fr.
Abstract
Background: Testosterone levels decline as men age, as does cognitive function. Whether there is more than a temporal relationship between testosterone and cognitive function is unclear. Chemical castration studies in men with prostate cancer suggest that low serum testosterone may be associated with cognitive dysfunction. Low testosterone levels have also been observed in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and mild cognitive impairment (MCI). This paper reviews the current clinical evidence of the relationship between serum testosterone levels and cognitive function in older men. Methods: A systematic literature search was conducted using PubMed and EMBASE to identify clinical studies and relevant reviews that evaluated cognitive function and endogenous testosterone levels or the effects of testosterone substitution in older men.
Results: Low levels of endogenous testosterone in healthy older men may be associated with poor performance on at least some cognitive tests. The results of randomized, placebo-controlled studies have been mixed, but generally indicate that testosterone substitution may have moderate positive effects on selective cognitive domains (e.g. spatial ability) in older men with and without hypogonadism. Similar results have been found in studies in patients with existing AD or MCI. Conclusions: Low endogenous levels of testosterone may be related to reduced cognitive ability, and testosterone substitution may improve some aspects of cognitive ability. Measurement of serum testosterone should be considered in older men with cognitive dysfunction. For men with both cognitive impairment and low testosterone, testosterone substitution may be considered. Large, longterm studies evaluating the effects of testosterone substitution on cognitive function in older men are warranted.
Age-related declines in cognitive function and testosterone
Cognitive ability, including memory, attention, language and visuospatial ability, declines as people age (1, 2). The prevalence of cognitive dysfunction in older adults is high, although estimates vary depending on the methodology and definitions used. Moderate to severe memory impairment has been estimated to occur in about 13% of adults aged ≥65 years and in 32% of adults aged ≥ 85 years (3). Nearly 40% of individuals aged 60-78 years were found to have age-associated memory impairment as defined by the National Institute of Mental Health criteria (4). However, about 25% of individuals aged 68-78 years were found to have ‘ageing-associated cognitive decline’ as defined by the more stringent criteria developed by the International Psychogeriatric Association (5). Estimates of the prevalence of mild cognitive impairment (MCI), a transitional state between normal ageing and Alzheimer’s disease (AD), range between 3 and 20% in adults aged ≥ 75 years (6).
Serum testosterone levels also decline as men age (7). Between the ages of 30 and 80 years, the mean free testosterone index (the ratio of serum total testosterone to sex-hormone-binding globulin (SHBG)) decreases by as much as 50% in men (8). Furthermore, approximately one in ten men over the age of 50 years, increasing to one in five over the age of 60 years, has hypogonadal levels of serum testosterone (9).
Some researchers have suggested that these age- related reductions in cognition and testosterone are more than temporally related (10), although others have argued that the current clinical evidence is inconclusive (11). Cognitive impairment is considered a component of late-onset hypogonadism, for which some men may receive testosterone substitution (2, 12, 13). This has raised many questions regarding the potential for hormone therapy, particularly testosterone substitution, to prevent or delay age-related cognitive impairment and possibly the onset of dementia. This article will review the current clinical evidence regarding the relationship between serum testosterone levels and cognitive function in older men.
Methods
The literature was searched using PubMed and EMBASE databases. General inclusion criteria included clinical trials, randomized controlled trials and review articles published in English in peer-reviewed priority journals after 1999 that included at least ten human male adult subjects. Keywords used in PubMed were the following: hypogonadism and (cognition or cognition disorders or cognitive therapy); hypogonadism and testosterone and (cognition or cognition disorders or cognitive therapy); andropause and (cognition or cognition disorders or cognitive therapy); andropause and testosterone and (cognition or cognition disorders or cognitive therapy) and testosterone and (cognition or cognition disorders or cognitive therapy). For EMBASE, keywords included the following: hypogonadism and (cognition or cognitive defect); hypogonadism and testosterone and (cognition or cognitive defect); (andropause or andropause-related disorder) and (cognition or cognitive defect); (andro- pause or andropause-related disorder) and testosterone and (cognition or cognitive defect) and testosterone and (cognition or cognitive defect). Clinical trials of patients with congenital endocrine abnormalities or of people seeking gender reassignment were excluded. For completeness, additional key trials known to the author that did not meet the initial search criteria were included.
Testosterone effects in the adult brain
Possible mechanisms to explain the relationship between testosterone and cognitive ability in older adults are based on preclinical observations of the neurotrophic and neuroprotective effects of testosterone (14, 15). In the brain, testosterone can be metabolized to dihydrotestosterone and bind to androgen receptors, or it can be converted to oestradiol by the enzyme aromatase. Both aromatase and androgen receptors are found in key regions in the brain involved in memory and learning, including the hippocampus and amygdala (14). Testosterone has been shown to increase concentrations of nerve growth factor (NGF) in the hippocampus and upregulate NGF receptors in the forebrain (16). Androgens can prevent N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor excitotoxicity in hippocampal neurons and promote fibre outgrowth and sprouting, which may help neurons recover after injury (17, 18). Neuroprotective effects against oxidative stress (19) and apoptosis (20) could also help protect the brain against accelerated age-related cognitive decline.
Testosterone may protect the brain against the development of AD by inhibiting two hallmarks of AD pathology: b-amyloid found in senile plaques and hyperphosphorylated tau found in neurofibrillary tangles. Testosterone has been shown to reduce b- amyloid secretion in rat cortical neurons by altering the processing of the amyloid precursor protein (21). In hippocampal neurons grown in culture, testosterone can reduce b-amyloid-induced neurotoxicity (22). In addition, testosterone has been shown to inhibit the hyperphosphorylation of tau in animal models (23).
Despite these observations, the biological effects of testosterone on the brain are far from fully understood. Further insight on the effects of testosterone substitution on neurological activity in different regions of the brain may be provided by advances in imaging techniques. For example, Zitzmann and colleagues (24) used 18F-deoxyglucose positron emission tomography to study cerebral glucose metabolism during a standardized mental rotation task in six hypogonadal men. Each patient performed the test before and during treatment with testosterone substitution. During testosterone substitution four patients exhibited improved visuospatial performance, which corresponded with enhanced cerebral glucose metabolism during the test (24). Using single photon emission computed tomography, Azad and colleagues (25) showed that cerebral perfusion was increased in the midbrain and the superior frontal gyrus after 3-5 weeks testosterone substitution in seven men with hypogonadism. After 12-14 weeks, increased perfusion was still observed in the midbrain as well as the midcingulate gyrus. Lastly, Park and colleagues (26) used blood oxygenation level- dependent (BOLD) functional magnetic resonance imaging to demonstrate differences in regional brain activation among 12 eugonadal sexually potent men and 2 hypogonadal impotent men in response to visual erotic stimulation. Activation of cerebral cortices in the two men with hypogonadism was low but was partially restored following testosterone substitution. Whether similar techniques could be used to assess differences in brain activity during cognitive tasks remains to be studied. These and other approaches could help to further establish the effects of testosterone on the brain in relation to cognitive ability.
Chemical castration and cognition
Surgical and chemical castration is used routinely in the treatment of prostate cancer. Some studies have evaluated cognitive ability during and following chemical castration (27-29). A study of 26 men with prostate cancer reported impaired visuomotor speed, attention and verbal recall following chemical castration, although recall of objects was improved after castration levels of serum testosterone were achieved by 6 months (27). In a study of 40 patients who underwent chemical castration for 36 weeks and were then observed for an additional 18 weeks, improvements in global cognitive function and word list recall seen only after treatment was discontinued (28). Lastly, in a study of 19 patients with prostate cancer who underwent chemical castration, scores for spatial rotation worsened, whereas scores for verbal memory improved during treatment (29). Compared with 15 healthy community-dwelling controls, however, no significant change was observed in verbal and spatial memory, executive functions or language. Taken together, this preliminary evidence suggests that artificially low-ering testosterone levels may impair performance in some cognitive domains, particularly spatial ability.
The relationship between serum levels of testosterone and cognitive function
Numerous studies have attempted to describe the relationship between serum testosterone levels and cognitive ability in younger adults. Although some studies have failed to find any relationship (30, 31), others have reported linear (32-34) or non-linear (35-38) relationships. Two studies of healthy young men and women have suggested a curvilinear (inverted U-shape) relationship between serum testosterone levels and spatial ability, whereby higher and lower levels of testosterone had the low spatial ability scores, whereas intermediate levels of testosterone were associated with better spatial performance (35, 36). Moffat and colleagues (3 7) reported a similar relationship, but only in right-handed subjects. In a cross-sectional study of men aged 40-80 years, Muller and colleagues (38) found a similar curvilinear relationship between bioavailable testosterone levels in serum, and memory performance and processing capacity/speed. Taken together, these studies suggest that a mid-range of serum testosterone levels may correspond with optimal cognitive performance. However, a plausible biological mechanism has not yet been proposed to adequately explain this phenomenon. In the study by Muller and colleagues (38), a positive linear relationship between bioavailable testosterone and cognitive ability was observed in the oldest patients (aged 70-80 years). Other studies have also reported a positive linear relationship in which higher testosterone levels were associated with improved spatial ability (32, 33). Based on a cross-sectional study of 56 men aged 21-84 years, Morley and colleagues (34) determined that bioavailable testosterone levels were positively associated with tests of verbal and non-verbal learning and memory.
Fewer studies have been conducted specifically in older men (Table 1) (2, 39-41). Two population-based studies found that bioavailable testosterone levels were positively associated with scores of general cognitive ability (40, 41). In a study of 310 older men aged ≥ 50 years (mean age 73 years), Yaffe and colleagues (40) measured serum concentrations of total and bioavailable testosterone and other hormonal factors. Cognitive function was assessed using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Trails B and Digit Symbol tests. Men with high levels of bioavailable testosterone were found to have better cognitive scores on all the three tests (P ≤ 0.001), while SHBG levels were negatively associated with cognitive test scores (P ≤ 0.001). Total serum testosterone was not associated with cognitive ability. The fact that bioavailable testosterone is more biologically active and more readily crosses the blood- brain barrier than bound testosterone may explain why bioavailable testosterone more strongly correlated with tests of cognitive ability than total serum testosterone.
Table 1. Summary of trials evaluating endogenous testosterone and cognitive function in elderly men.
In a population-based study of 547 men aged 59-89 years (mean age 70.2 years), Barrett-Connor and colleagues (41) measured serum testosterone levels between 1984 and 1987 and cognitive function using 12 neuropsychological tests between 1988 and 1991. Increased bioavailable testosterone was associated with improved scores on the Blessed Information-Memory- Concentration (BIMC) test and three measures of retrieval from the Buschke-Fuld Selective Reminding test. Curvilinear, U-shaped relationships were found between the total testosterone and the BIMC test and between the bioavailable testosterone and the ‘World’ Backwards component of the MMSE. Although the results of this study may have been limited by the fact that cognitive status was not assessed during the same period that testosterone levels were measured, the linear and the non-linear associations found suggest that optimal cognitive function may be related to a specific range of testosterone levels.
Studies that have assessed more specific domains of cognitive function in older men have produced mixed results, most likely due to differences in the tests of cognitive function used (2, 39). In a long-term longitudi-nal study, 407 elderly men aged 50-91 years at baseline (mean age 64.1 years) were assessed for cognitive status and testosterone levels over an average duration of 9.7 years (2). Tests of cognitive function and behaviour included verbal and visual memory, mental status, visuomotor scanning and attention, verbal knowledge/ language, visuospatial ability and symptoms of depression. Overall, an increased free testosterone index was associated with improved scores on visual and verbal memory, visuospatial function and visuomotor scanning. It was also related to less rapid decline in visual memory as assessed by the Benton Visual Retention test. Notably, men considered hypogonadal (n=149) had worse scores of visual and verbal memory, visuomotor scanning and visuospatial rotation compared with men considered eugonadal (n = 211). Men with hypogonadism also had faster rates of decline in visual memory.
In contrast, Fonda and colleagues (39) found no significant relationship between hormone levels and cognitive function in 981 men aged 48-80 years (mean age 62.7 years) using tests of working memory (Backward Digit Span test), speed/attention (Digit Symbol Substitution test) and spatial ability (Figural Relations test). Initially, free and total testosterone levels and other hormones were significantly related to performance on at least one out of the three cognitive domains tested. However, these relationships were no longer significant after adjusting for age, education level and physical status, with the exception of oestrone and cortisol levels, which had a negative relationship with test results. Based on the cognitive domains tested in this study, the authors concluded that the hormones do not mediate age-related cognitive decline (39).
Overall, it appears that low endogenous levels of testosterone may be associated with reduced cognitive ability in older men, although the current evidence is inconclusive and the exact relationship is far from being fully understood. The different results of the studies conducted to date may be attributed to their small sample size or to differences in study populations, cognitive tests used and method of testosterone assessment.
Testosterone substitution in men without cognitive impairment
Does testosterone substitution improve cognitive ability in older men? The results of some placebo-controlled studies suggest that it may have a positive effect on some cognitive domains, particularly spatial ability and memory (Table 2) (42-44). In a 3-month placebo-controlled study of 56 healthy men aged 60-75 years (mean age 67.4 years), testosterone substitution was found to improve spatial cognition but not verbal and visual memory (42). In a 1-month study in 19 healthy men aged 61-75 years (mean age 67.5 years), working memory improved significantly in the patients receiving testosterone substitution (43). Lastly, in a 6-week placebo-controlled study in 25 healthy older men aged 50-80 years, testosterone substitution was found to improve spatial cognition as well as verbal memory (44). This was the first study to report a significant improvement in verbal memory with testosterone substitution in older men, which was hypothesized to be a result of conversion of testosterone to oestradiol in the brain. This hypothesis was confirmed by a later study in which 60 healthy older men aged 50-90 years were randomized to weekly testosterone injection (100 mg) plus oral placebo, testosterone injection plus an oral aromatase inhibitor (to prevent conversion to oestradiol) or saline injection plus oral placebo for 6 weeks (45). Spatial memory improved in both groups that received testosterone. However, verbal memory improved in those who received testosterone only but not in those who received an aromatase inhibitor, suggesting that oestradiol which has been converted from exogenous testosterone is responsible for improved verbal memory (45).
Table 2. Summary of placebo-controlled trials of testosterone substitution in elderly men.
Other placebo-controlled studies have failed to find any significant effect of testosterone substitution on cognitive ability in older men (Table 2) (46-48). One initial study of 30 healthy older men found that a single dose of testosterone had no effect on verbal or spatial tasks and had a negative effect on verbal fluency after 5 days (46). However, this study may have been too short to capture a meaningful effect on cognition. A 12-month study of 67 men aged 65-87 years (mean age 76 years) with bioavailable testosterone levels < 128ng/dl also found no difference in overall cognitive ability with testosterone substitution compared with placebo (47). Trails B scores improved from baseline in the testosterone group, but the difference was not significantly different from the change in scores in the placebo group (47). In a similar 12-month study in 32 men aged ≥50 years with bioavailable testosterone levels ≤ 60 ng/dl, Sih and colleagues (48) found that testosterone substitution had no significant effect on scores of verbal or non-verbal memory. However, visuospatial ability was not measured specifically in this study.
Overall, the results of these studies suggest that further investigation of the effects of testosterone substitution on cognition is warranted. Although some studies have reported no benefit with testosterone substitution, others have found significant improvement in some scores of cognitive ability indicating that testosterone may have selective effects on certain cognitive domains. It may also indicate that some patients may derive more benefit than others. In particular, it should be noted that baseline levels of serum testosterone varied considerably among the studies. Therefore, further evaluation of the effects of testosterone substitution on specific cognitive domains in a carefully selected patient population of older men is justified.
Testosterone substitution in men with cognitive impairment
Several studies have determined that men with AD have reduced levels of endogenous total or bioavailable testosterone (49-51). AD is characterized by senile plaques and neurofibrillary tangles in the brain. Senile plaques are composed of aggregated b-amyloid peptide, and neurofibrillary tangles are composed of abnormal filaments consisting primarily of hyperphosphorylated tau protein (21, 23). Testosterone has been associated with reduced plasma concentrations of b-amyloid peptide in clinical studies (52, 53) and reduced tau phosphorylation in preclinical models (23), suggesting that testosterone may protect against the development of AD.
Recently, much attention has been focused on identify-ing patients with MCI who may be at risk of developing AD. An estimated 6-25% of people with MCI will develop AD annually compared with a conversion rate in the generalpopulationof0.2-3.9% (54). MCI is characterized by significant impairment of short-term memory with little or no functional loss (55). It has been associated with significant neuronal loss in the hippocampus, reduced blood flow in the subiculum, and increases in b-amyloid depositions and tau protein concentrations (56, 57). Estimates of the prevalence of MCI range widely depending on the diagnostic criteria used. In an analysis of 1045 community-dwelling adults aged ≥75 years who participated in the Leipzig Longitudinal Study of the Aged (LEILA75 + ), prevalence rates for MCI ranged from 3 to 20% (6). The prevalence was 13.7% in a study of 2551 community-dwelling adults in Australia aged 60-64 years, but rates varied up to sixfold depending on the diagnostic criteria applied (58). Improved ways to identify and treat MCI may avoid or delay the onset of AD.
The clearest evidence regarding the role of testosterone in the early development of AD comes from the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (59). In this longitudinal study, 574 men without AD aged 32-87 years at baseline were followed for a mean of 19.1 years (range, 4-37 years). The impact of testosterone level on risk of developing AD was assessed at 2, 5 and 10 years before diagnosis. For every 10-unit (nmol/nmol) increase in the free testosterone index, the risk of developing AD was found to be reduced by 26%. The fact that low testosterone levels could predict the development of AD up to 10 years in advance clearly indicates that testosterone levels are often reduced prior to the development of AD and may coincide with the onset of MCI.
The effects of testosterone substitution in men with MCI or AD have been evaluated by four small placebo-controlled trials (Table 3) (60-63). The largest of these studies involved 32 men aged 63-85 years with MCI (n = 17) or AD (n = 15) (60). Mean baseline testosterone levels ranged from 9 to 14 nmol/l in the various demographic groups. After 6 weeks, patients given testosterone substitution had significantly better scores of spatial memory and constructional abilities than those given placebo. There was also a significant difference in verbal memory scores between treatment groups, due primarily to a worsening of scores in the placebo group; scores remained generally unchanged in the testosterone substitution group. Tan and Pu (61) conducted a pilot study in ten men with newly diagnosed AD and hypogonadism (serum total testosterone < 7 nmol/l). They reported improved visuospatial ability scores after 1 year of testosterone substitution compared with placebo. In contrast, Kenny and colleagues (62) found no effect of testosterone substitution on cognitive ability after 3 months in 11 men with early cognitive decline and hypogonadism (serum bioavailable testosterone < 128ng/dl). However, the tests used in this study focused primarily on domains that were unlikely to be affected by testosterone substitution, such as attention, language and motor coordination. Lastly, Lu and colleagues (63) evaluated the effects of testosterone substitution or placebo on cognition and quality of life in 16 men with mild AD and 22 healthy controls. No significant differences in cognitive ability were found between treatment groups. However, patients with and without AD who received testosterone had numerical improvements or less decline in scores of visuospatial functions over the course of the study. Notably, the magnitude in change in testosterone levels from baseline was positively associated with visuospatial recognition scores indicating that men with lower endogenous testosterone levels derived the most benefit from testosterone substitution.
Table 3. Summary of placebo-controlled trials evaluating testosterone substitution in men with MCI or AD.
It is difficult to draw conclusions from these small studies that differed substantially with regard to study population and methodology. However, the potential of testosterone substitution to improve spatial ability deserves further investigation in this setting. Memory loss and spatial disorientation are two early hallmarks of AD. Since spatial memory and ability are essential for navigating complex environments, deficiencies can lead to environmental disorientation, which can rapidly become a considerable burden on caregivers. Interven-tions that improve navigational abilities or delay the onset of environmental disorientation could therefore have a substantial impact on healthcare burden as well as patient quality of life.
Summary of clinical trial review
There is a strong biological rationale to support the potential protective effects of testosterone against the age-related cognitive decline and the development of dementia in men. Cognitive impairment has been observed in men with prostate cancer undergoing chemical castration, further indicating that testosterone may play an important part in cognitive function. Low serum testosterone levels have been associated with low performance in cognitive tests and at least one large longitudinal study has demonstrated that low testoster-one levels are often present up to 10 years prior to the onset of AD. In older healthy men, lower levels of endogenous testosterone may be associated with poor performance on at least some cognitive tests. Lastly, the results of randomized, placebo-controlled studies indicate that testosterone substitution may have moderate positive effects on selective cognitive domains in older men with and without hypogonadism. Similar results have been found in studies conducted in patients with existing AD or MCI. Since many of these studies were small and varied considerably in their design and methodology, definitive conclusions should be withheld until further evidence is available.
Management of patients with late-onset hypogonadism and cognitive dysfunction
The International Society of Andrology, the International Society for the Study of the Aging Male and the European Association of Urology have recently jointly produced recommendations for the management of late-onset hypogonadism (12). According to the recommendations, late-onset hypogonadism can be accompanied by changes in mood with concomitant reductions in intellectual activity, cognitive functions and spatial orientation ability, as well as fatigue, depressed mood and irritability. The most easily recognized features of late-onset hypogonadism are reduced libido and erection dysfunction. Changes in body composition, such as decreased lean body mass and increased visceral fat, are also associated with late- onset hypogonadism.
It is difficult to establish universal cut-off levels of serum testosterone that correspond with clinical late- onset hypogonadism. Symptoms typically manifest when total serum testosterone levels are between 8 and 12 nmol/l (12). When alternative causes of these symptoms have been excluded, testosterone substitution may be considered. Men with serum levels of total testosterone < 8 nmol/l or free testosterone < 180pmol/l generally require testosterone substitution. Before initiating testosterone substitution, men aged ≥45 years should undergo digital rectal examination (DRE) and prostate-specific antigen (PSA) testing to establish prostate health. Men with confirmed or suspected prostate or breast cancer should not receive testosterone substitution. DRE and PSA testing should be conducted at quarterly intervals during the first year of treatment and annually thereafter.
Cognitive dysfunction can be detected with the help of multiple tools of varying specificity. General prac-titioners, endocrinologists, urologists and other special-ists may use non-specific measures, such as the MMSE, the Kokmen Short Test of Mental Status, the 7-Min Screen or the Memory Impairment Screen to detect possible cognitive impairment (54). To confirm the diagnosis, a general neurologist or geriatrician may use more specific tests for dementia, such as the Diagnostic and the Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (64). To determine whether cognitive impairment may be related to low testosterone levels, neuropsychologists may test specifically the relevant cognitive domains, such as visuospatial memory, spatial ability and non-verbal working memory.
Identification of cognitive impairment in older adults is only relevant if effective interventions are available (54). Treatment strategies for cognitive impairment consist of pharmacological and non-pharmacological approaches. The number of non-pharmacological therapies has expanded to include not only standard approaches, such as behavioural therapy and reality orientation, but also brief psychotherapy and alternative therapies, such as art or music therapy. Pharmacological approaches consist mainly of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors, which have a modest effect on symptoms but do not prevent disease progression, and non-specific therapies, such as neuroleptics and other sedatives. Identifying new treatment approaches that safely and effectively prevent or delay the onset of dementia is the subject of ongoing investigation.
Final comment
Despite decades of research on the relationship between testosterone and cognitive function in men, many questions remain. In particular, the appropriate study population has yet to be established. Some have suggested that more severely hypogonadal men may derive more benefit from testosterone substitution than mildly hypogonadal or eugonadal men (63). However, others have in fact suggested the opposite that eugonadal men may derive more benefit because longterm hypogonadism may confer a lack of sensitivity of androgen receptors in the brain (15). The appropriate age has also been questioned in a review of studies conducted in elderly men on cognition and testosterone (evaluating endogenous levels or the effects of substitution), Hogervorst and colleagues (15) noted a trend in which a positive effect of testosterone on cognitive function was observed, primarily in studies with a younger median age (roughly < 72 years). This observation may not only explain some of the mixed results reported from various studies but also suggest a therapeutic strategy whereby testosterone substitution would be considered for younger patients. In addition to issues of study population, the optimal dose and type of testosterone and the duration of therapy needed to provide optimal effects on cognition must also be established.
All of the issues mentioned above point to the need for more clinical data from well-designed controlled clinical trials using established methods of measuring cognitive ability and serum testosterone levels. In the meantime, endocrinologists and urologists should be aware of the potential cognitive impairment in men with low serum testosterone. Consultation with a neurologist or geriatrician may help to identify and manage symptoms related to cognitive decline. In addition, neurologists and geriatricians should be aware of the possibility of hypogonadism in elderly men with cognitive impairment, and should consider measuring serum testosterone concentrations in these patients.
References
CT Liverman & DG Blazer (ed.). Testosterone and Aging. Clinical Research Directions. Washington, DC, USA: The National Academies Press, 2004
Moffat SD, Zonderman AB, Metter EJ, Blackman MR, Harman SM & Resnick SM. Longitudinal assessment of serum free testosterone concentration predicts memory performance and cognitive status in elderly men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 2002 87 5001-5007.
Federal Interagency Forum on Aging-Related Statistics 2002
Available at:http://www.agingstats.gov/chartbook2004/
CBhealth%20status.xls. Accessed January 2006.
Koivisto K, Reinikainen KJ, Hanninen T, Vanhanen M, Helkala EL, Mykkanen L, Laakso M, Pyorala K & Riekkinen PJ. Prevalence of age-associated memory impairment in a randomly selected population from eastern Finland. Neurology 1995 45 741-747.
Hanninen T, Koivisto K, Reinikainen KJ, Helkala EL, Soininen H, Mykkanen L, Laakso M & Riekkinen PJ. Prevalence of ageing- associated cognitive decline in an elderly population. Age and Ageing 1996 25 201-205.
Busse A, Bischkopf J, Riedel-Heller SG & Angermeyer MC. Mild cognitive impairment: prevalence and incidence according to different diagnostic criteria. British Journal of Psychiatry 2003 182 449-454.
Leifke E, Gorenoi V, Wichers C, Von Zur Muhlen A, Von Buren E & Brabant G. Age-related changes of serum sex hormones, insulinlike growth factor-1 and sex- hormone binding globulin levels in men: cross-sectional data from a healthy male cohort. Clinical Endocrinology (Oxford) 2000 53 689-695.
Lamberts SW, van den Beld AW & van der Lely AJ. The endocrinology of aging. Science 1997 278 419-424.
Harman SM, Metter EJ, Tobin JD, Pearson J & Blackman MR. Longitudinal effects of aging on serum total and free testosterone levels in healthy men. Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Journalof Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 2001 86 724-731.
Greene R. Sex steroids and brain functioning in the aging male. Aging Male 2001 4 39-43.
Almeida OP & Flicker L. Testosterone and dementia: too much ado about too little data. Journal of the British Menopause Society 2003 9 107-110.
Nieschlag E, Swerdloff R, Behre HM, Gooren LJ, Kaufman JM, Legros JJ, Lunenfeld B, Morley JE, Schulman C, Wang C, Weidner W & Wu FC. Investigation, treatment and monitoring of late-onset hypogonadism in males. ISA, ISSAM, and EAU recommendations. European Urology 2005 48 1-4.
Sherwin BB. Steroid hormones and cognitive functioning in aging men: a mini-review. Journal of Molecular Neuroscience 2003 20 385-393.
Janowsky JS. Thinking with your gonads: testosterone and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2006 10 77-82.
Hogervorst E, Bandelow S & Moffat SD. Increasing testosterone levels and effects on cognitive functions in elderly men and women: a review. Current Drug Targets. CNS and Neurological Disorders 2005 4 531-540.
Tirassa P, Thiblin I, Agren G, Vigneti E, Aloe L & Stenfors C. Highdose anabolic androgenic steroids modulate concentrations of nerve growth factor and expression of its low affinity receptor (p75-NGFr) in male rat brain. Journal of Neuroscience Research 1997 47 198-207.
Pouliot WA, Handa RJ & Beck SG. Androgen modulates N-methyl- D-aspartate-mediated depolarization in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal cells. Synapse 1996 23 10-19.
Morse JK, DeKosky ST & Scheff SW Neurotrophic effects of steroids on lesion-induced growth in the hippocampus. II. Hormone replacement. Experimental Neurology 1992 118 47-52.
Ahlbom E, Prins GS & Ceccatelli S. Testosterone protects cerebellar granule cells from oxidative stress-induced cell death through a receptor mediated mechanism. Brain Research 2001 892 255-262.
Hammond J, Le Q, Goodyer C, Gelfand M, Trifiro M & LeBlanc A. Testosterone-mediated neuroprotection through the androgen receptor in human primary neurons. Journal of Neurochemistry 2001 77 1319-1326.
Gouras GK, Xu H, Gross RS, Greenfield JP, Hai B, Wang R & Greengard P. Testosterone reduces neuronal secretion of Alzheimer’s beta-amyloid peptides. PNAS 2000 97 1202-1205.
Pike CJ. Testosterone attenuates beta-amyloid toxicity in cultured hippocampal neurons. Brain Research 2001 919 160-165.
Papasozomenos SC & Shanavas A. Testosterone prevents the heat shock-induced overactivation of glycogen synthase kinase-3beta but not of cyclin-dependent kinase 5 and c-Jun NH2-terminal kinase and concomitantly abolishes hyperphosphorylation of tau: implications for Alzheimer’s disease. PNAS 2002 99 1140-1145.
Zitzmann M, Weckesser M, Schober O & Nieschlag E. Changes in cerebral glucose metabolism and visuospatial capacity in hypogo- nadal males under testosterone substitution therapy. Experimental and Clinical Endocrinology andDiabetes 2001 109 302-304.
Azad N, Pitale S, Barnes WE & Friedman N. Testosterone treatment enhances regional brain perfusion in hypogonadal men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 2003 88 3064-3068.
Park K, Seo JJ, Kang HK, Ryu SB, Kim HJ & Jeong GW. A new potential of blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) functional MRI for evaluating cerebral centers of penile erection. International Journal of Impotence Research 2001 13 73-81.
Salminen EK, Portin RI, Koskinen A, Helenius H & Nurmi M. Associations between serum testosterone fall and cognitive function in prostate cancer patients. Clinical Cancer Research 2004 10 7575-7582.
Almeida OP, Waterreus A, Spry N, Flicker L & Martins RN. One year follow-up study of the association between chemical castration, sex hormones, beta-amyloid, memory and depression in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2004 29 1071-1081.
Cherrier MM, Rose AL & Higano C. The effects of combined androgen blockade on cognitive function during the first cycle of intermittent androgen suppression in patients with prostate cancer. Journal of Urology 2003 170 1808-1811.
McKeever WF & Deyo RA. Testosterone, dihydrotestosterone, and spatial task performances of males. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 1990 28 305-308.
Gordon HW & Lee PA. A relationship between gonadotropins and visuospatial function. Neuropsychologia 1986 24 563-576.
Christiansen K & Knussmann R. Sex hormones and cognitive functioning in men. Neuropsychobiology 1987 18 27-36.
Silverman I, Kastuk D, Choi J & Phillips K. Testosterone levels and spatial ability in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1999 24 813-822.
Morley JE, Kaiser F, Raum WJ, Perry HM, III, Flood JF, Jensen J, Silver AJ & Roberts E. Potentially predictive and manipulable blood serum correlates of aging in the healthy human male: progressive decreases in bioavailable testosterone, dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate, and the ratio of insulin-like growth factor 1 to growth hormone. PNAS 1997 94 7537-7542.
Shute VJ, Pellegrino JW, Hubert L & Reynolds RW The relationship between androgen levels and human spatial ability. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 1983 21 465-468.
Gouchie C & Kimura D. The relationship between testosterone levels and cognitive ability patterns. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1991 16 323-334.
Moffat SD & Hampson E. A curvilinear relationship between testosterone and spatial cognition in humans: possible influence of hand preference. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1996 21 323-337.
Muller M, Aleman A, Grobbee DE, de Haan EHF & van der Schouw YT Endogenous sex hormone levels and cognitive function in aging men. Is there an optimal level? Neurology 2005 64 866-871.
Fonda SJ, Bertrand R, O’Donnell A, Longcope C & McKinlay JB. Age, hormones, and cognitive functioning among middle-aged and elderly men: cross-sectional evidence from the Massachusetts Male Aging Study Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 2005 60 385-390.
Yaffe K, Lui L-Y, Zmuda J & Cauley J. Sex hormones and cognitive function in older men. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 2002 50 707-712.
Barrett-Connor E, Goodman-Gruen D & Patay B. Endogenous sex hormones and cognitive function in older men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1999 84 3681-3685.
Janowsky JS, Oviatt SK & Orwoll ES. Testosterone influences spatial cognition in older men. Behavioral Neuroscience 1994 108 325-332.
Janowsky JS, Chavez B & Orwoll E. Sex steroids modify working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2000 12 407-414.
Cherrier MM, Asthana S, Plymate S, Baker L, Matsumoto AM, Peskind E, Raskind MA, Brodkin K, Bremner W, Petrova A, LaTendresse S & Craft S. Testosterone supplementation improves spatial and verbal memory in healthy older men. Neurology 2001 57 80-88.
Cherrier MM, Matsumoto AM, Amory JK, Ahmed S, Bremner W, Peskind ER, Raskind MA, Johnson M & Craft S. The role of aromatization in testosterone supplementation: effects on cognition in older men. Neurology 2005 64 290-296.
Wolf OT, Preut R, Hellhammer DH, Kudielka BM, Schiirmeyer TH & Kirschbaum C. Testosterone and cognition in elderly men: a single testosterone injection blocks the practice effect in verbal fluency, but has no effect on spatial or verbal memory. Biological Psychiatry 2000 47 650-654.
Kenny AM, Bellantonio S, Gruman CA, Acosta RD & Prestwood KM. Effects of transdermal testosterone on cognitive function and health perception in older men with low bioavailable testosterone levels. Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 2002 57 M321-M325.
Sih R, Morley JE, Kaiser FE, Perry HM III, Patrick P & Ross C. Testosterone replacement in older hypogonadal men: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 1997 82 1661-1667.
Paoletti AM, Congia S, Lello S, Tedde D, Orru M, Pistis M, Pilloni M, Zedda P Loddo A & Melis GB. Low androgenization index in elderly women and elderly men with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 2004 62 301-303.
Hogervorst E, Bandelow S, Combrinck M & Smith AD. Low free testosterone is an independent risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease. Experimental Gerontology 2004 39 1633-1639.
Okun MS, DeLong MR, Hanfelt J, Gearing M & Levey A. Plasma testosterone levels in Alzheimer and Parkinson diseases. Neurology 2004 62 411-413.
Gillett MG, Martins RN, Clarnette RM, Chubb SA, Bruce DG & Yeap BB. Relationship between testosterone, sex hormone binding globulin and plasma amyloid beta peptide 40 in older men with subjective memory loss or dementia. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease 2003 5 267-269.
Gandy S, Almeida OP, Fonte J, Lim D, Waterrus A, Spry N, Flicker L & Martins RN. Chemical andropause and amyloid-beta peptide. JAMA 2001 285 2195-2196.
Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, Mohs RC, Morris JC, Rabins PV, Ritchie K, Rossor M, Thal L & Winblad B. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology 2001 58 1985-1992.
Tan RS, Pu SJ & Culberson JW. Role of androgens in mild cognitive impairment and possible interventions during andropause. Medical Hypotheses 2003 60 448-452.
Mufson EJ, Chen EY, Cochran EJ, Beckett LA, Bennett DA & Kordower JH. Entorhinal cortex beta-amyloid load in individuals with mild cognitive impairment. Experimental Neurology 1999 158 469-490.
Mitchell TW, Mufson EJ, Schneider JA, Cochran EJ, Nissanov J, Han LY, Bienias JL, Lee VM, Trojanowski JQ, Bennett DA & Arnold SE. Parahippocampal tau pathology in healthy aging, mild cognitive impairment, and early Alzheimer’s disease. Annals of Neurology 2002 51 182-189.
Kumar R, Dear KB, Christensen H, Ilschner S, Jorm AF, Meslin C, Rosenman SJ & Sachdev PS. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in 60- to 64-year-old community dwelling individuals: the Personality and Total Health through Life 60+ Study Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 2005 19 67-74.
Moffat SD, Zonderman AB, Metter EJ, Kawas C, Blackman MR, Harman SM & Resnick SM. Free testosterone and risk for Alzheimer disease in older men. Neurology 2004 62 188-193.
Cherrier MM, Matsumoto AM, Amory JK, Asthana S, Bremner W, Peskind ER, Raskind MA & Craft S. Testosterone improves spatial memory in men with Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 2005 64 2063-2068.
Tan RS & Pu SJ. A pilot study on the effects of testosterone in hypogonadal aging male patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Aging Male 2003 6 13-17.
Kenny AM, Fabregas G, Song C, Biskup B & Bellantonio S. Effects of testosterone on behavior, depression, and cognitive function in older men with mild cognitive loss. Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 2004 59 75-78.
Lu PH, Masterman DA, Mulnard R, Cotman C, Miller B, Yaffe K, RebackE, Porter V SwerdloffR & Cummings JL. Effects of testosterone on cognition and mood in male patients with mild Alzheimer disease and healthy elderly men. Archives of Neurology 2006 63 177-185.
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. edn 4 Washington, DC, USA: American Psychiatric Association, 2000. Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR).