Barbara Fischer, PsyD, Carey Gleason, PhD. and Sanjay Asthana, M.
Abstract
OBJECTIVE:
Results of the Women's Health Initiative (WHI) and Women's Health Initiative Memory Study (WHIMS) suggested that hormone therapy (HT) may be detrimental to cognitive health. This article reviews clinical studies that address issues relevant to those results.
DESIGN:
Literature review.
INTERVENTION(S):
A search of Pubmed and Web of Science was conducted using the search terms HT and cognition, HT and mood. Clinical and observational studies were selected if they were published after the year 2000. Theories of HT mechanisms of action, pharmacology, biology, and observational and clinical trials are discussed.
RESULT(S):
Although observational and clinical trials show conflicting findings, methodologic considerations must be acknowledged. HT formulation and dose, route of administration, timing of initiation, length of treatment, and health of participants all contribute to inconsistencies in results. Transdermal estradiol and micronized progesterone administered at time of menopause are generally associated with cognitive and affective benefit.
CONCLUSION(S):
At the present time, results from existing studies are equivocal regarding the benefits of HT on cognition and affect. Future studies, such as the Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Study (KEEPS), should address methodologic inconsistencies to provide clearer answers to this important question.
A woman born in the United States in the second half of the twentieth century is expected to live 71–83 years (1), such that 20 to 30 years may be lived after menopause. Precipitated by a substantial decline in circulating endogenous estrogen, postmenopause is by no means a disease state. However, relative to pre-menopause, it represents a distinctly different physiologic stage, for which there may be systemic consequences (e.g., osteoporosis). Indeed, estrogen, a complex gonadal hormone with receptor sites throughout the brain and periphery (2), appears to facilitate numerous functions, including autonomic regulation, mood, and cognition. As a result, hormone therapy (HT) has been widely prescribed to partially replace naturally occurring declines in hormones (both estrogen and progesterone) at menopause.
The general term estrogen refers to three different hormones, estradiol (E2), estrone (E1), and estriol (E3). E2, the most potent form of the hormone, is predominant before menopause, whereas E1 is the primary form postmenopausally. During pregnancy, E3 is produced by the placenta and becomes predominant (3). Before menopause, E2 and E1 are produced by the ovaries and circulate freely throughout the body, dynamically interacting with each other, such that E1 may metabolize into E2 and vice versa (4). After menopause, however, E2 is produced from aromatization of E1 and androstenedione in adipose tissue, and the volume of circulating E2 drops to ∼10% of earlier levels (5). In contrast, E1 declines to a much less degree, and continues to be synthesized by fatty tissue (6). Thus the ratio of E2 to estrone changes markedly at menopause.
Neurobiology of estrogen's effects on cognition and mood
Estrogen works synergistically with many biologic systems to promote physical, cognitive, and affective function. Basic science reveals that administration of estrogen (E2 alone and E2 plus P) results in increased levels of antioxidants, reduces free radicals, and substantially lowers oxidative damage to mitochondrial DNA (7). Critical to neuronal health, estrogen also regulates glucose and oxidative metabolism, mitochondrial function, and promotes adenosine triphosphate (ATP) (8). Indeed, declines in these processes are characteristic of neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer disease (AD) (9).
Estrogen promotes neural plasticity both directly and indirectly. Normal aging is associated with a decline in dendritic spines and synapses in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and hippocampus (10 [review], 11). In animal studies, impaired memory function is correlated with reductions in hippocampal dendritic spines (12). Estrogen treatment (estrogen α and β receptors) promotes the growth of long thin spines in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex (13, 14, 15). Higher levels of presynaptic estrogen α receptors are associated with stronger memory performance in ovariectomized animals treated with exogenous estrogen (15). Neurotrophins, such as nerve growth factor (NGF) and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), represent another mechanism of neural plasticity in the brain. These substances facilitate the growth of dendritic spines in the hippocampus (13 [review]). In vitro investigations support a favorable effect of estrogens on the activity of neurotropins (16, 17). By increasing hippocampal BDNF levels, estrogen transiently increases dendritic spines, thereby potentiating opportunity for increased connectivity and plasticity (14).
Estradiol has been shown to favorably influence several factors potentially altering the health of the neurovascular unit, including antioxidative protection from by-products of mitochondrial energy metabolism (reactive oxygen species [ROS] and peroxides) (18), and reduced thrombolic (19) and endothelial inflammatory activity (20, 21). Additionally, estrogens exert a number of more general neuroprotective actions to maintain the health of the cerebrovasculature. For example, E2 reportedly prevents glutamate-induced excitotoxicity (22, 23), protects neurons from ischemic insults (24), and offers antioxidant protection against several sources of cell damage and death (25, 26 [review]). By facilitating antiapoptotic actions of Bcl proteins, estrogens may also protect neurons from toxin-induced and ischemic brain injury (27, 28). Like other actions of the estrogens, characteristic of the substrate to which the hormones are applied, age appears to influence their efficacy. Preliminary evidence suggests that antiinflammatory and neuroprotective effects of the estrogens may be suppressed or abolished in aged animals (29, 30).
Estrogen also interacts with a number of neurotransmitters affecting cognition and mood. Animal research reveals that long-term use of estrogen prevented declines in cholinergic nerve cell fiber length and density at menopause (31). In humans, the negative effect of cholinergic challenge on cognitive performance and neural activation patterns was counteracted by prior exposure to E2 (32, 33). Of note, naturally occurring higher levels of E1 were associated with poorer cognition, specifically working memory performance (33).
Estrogen modulates mood via the serotonergic system at the cellular and synaptic levels (34, 35). Moreover, although tryptophan deletion reduced neural activation during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during a verbal working memory task, women who received pretreatment with transdermal E2 displayed less signal change/activation disruption (36). Estrogen contributes to up-regulation of serotonin 5HT-1 receptors and to down-regulation of 5HT-2 receptors and monoamine oxidase activity (37, 38). A serotonin agonist, estrogen responds via multiple mechanisms in brain regions supporting mood regulation (35, 37).
Implications of the Women's Health Initiative Memory Study
Before publication of the Women's Health Initiative (WHI) and Women's Health Initiative Memory Study (WHIMS) (39, 40, 41, 42), HT was thought to be neuroprotective, and a large body of basic science and generally supportive epidemiologic evidence suggested that HT reduced the risk of AD by ∼50% (43, 44 [reviews]). Approach to HT changed radically, however, following the results of the WHI and WHIMS. The largest clinical trials of HT ever conducted, these studies revealed an increased risk of cancer, dementia, and cognitive decline with prolonged administration of conjugated equine estrogen (CEE), and the studies were discontinued. Additionally, the Heart and Estrogen/Progestin Replacement Study (HERS), an investigation of long-term effects of HT on cognition and physical health, produced consistent results, indicating that long-term use of CEE was associated with poorer cognitive performance (45). In the wake of the WHI and HERS publications, there was an abrupt reversal of opinion, leading many to dismiss the previous epidemiologic evidence as spurious.
Although WHIMS and HERS were pivotal studies, closer evaluation suggested methodologic concerns that might explain at least partially explain their results. Both studies used orally administered opposed CEE (Prempro), which exerts different effects on the CNS than unopposed transdermal estrogen. Additionally, participants in both studies were older women over the age of 65 years. Various theories were offered to help assimilate the conflicting findings. We review several of these below.
Critical Window Theory
Differences in cognitive response to HT have been identified depending on women's age or timing of HT initiation. These discrepancies support the notion of a critical window during which HT may be beneficially initiated. The theory posits that HT administered at or around the time of menopause may elicit improved cognition, while HT initiated ≥5 years after menopause may engender no cognitive benefit or even cognitive detriment. Indeed a number of studies have revealed a positive association between early initiation of HT and cognition (2, 46 [reviews]). For example, exposure to HT at perimenopause resulted in improved memory and hippocampal function later in life (47). The REMEMBER pilot observational study revealed that early HT initiators demonstrated stronger global cognition and executive function than later initiators (48). Additionally, shorter time between menopause and initiation of HT was associated with larger hippocampal volume, bilaterally (49). Support for the critical window theory has not been uniformly positive, however. A recent study found no evidence of influence of time since menopause on cognitive outcomes (50). Moreover, a report on prerandomization data from the Early Versus Late Intervention Trial (ELITE), specifically investigating the effect of length of time in menopause on cognition, found no influence of time since menopause on the relationship between verbal memory and endogenous concentrations of free E2 (51). In light of these equivocal findings, additional research is needed to investigate the influence of time since menopause on HT efficacy.
Healthy Cell Bias Theory
A related framework with which to understand conflicting data of earlier clinical studies is the “healthy cell bias theory,” proposed by Brinton and colleagues. This theory purports that women who use HT during the menopausal transition may be systematically healthier than nonusers, and it is these fundamental differences that result in HT's salutary effects (9, 52). Specifically, evidence is emerging that the health of the neuronal substrate may influence the effect of estrogen exposure such that the same group of compounds, the estrogens, could be both beneficial and harmful, depending upon when during the neuronal life time the estrogens are initiated. In healthy neurons, estrogen appears to provide multiple neuroprotective benefits, as described above, including increased mitochondrial respiration and ATP generation, while also protecting cells by improving the cell's tolerance for calcium influx and triggering antioxidant actions (8). These actions may sustain cardiovascular and cognitive health (8, 9). However, in diseased cells, calcium homeostasis is disrupted, and estrogen-induced calcium influx now becomes deleterious to the neuron incapable of maintaining calcium homeostasis (9). Thus, although women may benefit from HT before cells are exposed to neuropathologic changes associated with disease (53), once disease processes affect cells, HT may not confer cognitive benefit. Indeed, as neurologic health deteriorates, estrogenic effects may facilitate disease pathogenesis (9).
Differences in HT Formulation
As noted above, differences in hormone formulation may result in divergent cognitive and affective outcomes. Estrogen preparations are available in a variety of forms. Transdermal estrogen is composed of E2, whereas oral CEE, a formulation of at least ten different hormones, is composed mainly of E1 (54). Although both forms alleviate menopausal symptoms, HT utilizing E2 more effectively recalibrates the E2:E1 ratio to approximate premenopausal levels. Moreover, oral CEE is metabolized by the liver which alters the E1:E2 ratio, and introduces risk of venous thromboembolic complications (55). Oral HT also increases binding glycoproteins that lower levels of biologically active free E2 (56). In contrast, transdermal E2 bypasses hepatic metabolism, avoiding these potential complications.
Differences in Progesterone Preparations
Progesterone, synthesized by the ovaries and adrenal glands, also has widely distributed receptors. Among its multiple roles is to function in an antagonistic manner to estrogen, modulating estrogen's trophic effects (57). In women who still have a uterus, HT is prescribed in combination form (estrogen together with progesterone) to counteract the potential for endometrial cancer which estrogen would otherwise facilitate. As with estrogen, progesterone may be taken in different forms. Medroxyprogesterone acetate (MPA), commonly prescribed with CEE in the HT Prempro, is a synthetic progestin that has been found to ameliorate the potential for cognitive benefit engendered in other hormones such as E2 (58 [review]). Prempro has been associated with numerous side effects, including heart disease, adverse changes in lipid metabolism, and thromboembolic processes (59). In contrast, micronized progestin (MP), bioidentical to the progesterone produced in the ovaries, has not been associated with such side effects. Taken together, evidence suggests that the type of hormone preparation utilized may significantly influence outcomes.
Hormone therapy and cognition
Observational Trials
Despite findings from earlier epidemiologic studies failing to document cognitive-enhancing effects of estrogen and reduced risk of AD (60), a number of studies have reported salutary effects of HT on cognition. Early results of the Cache County Study found that use of HT for ≥10 years was associated with a 2.5 times lower incidence of AD compared with men (61). More recent results from the same study revealed that any HT initiated within 5 years of menopause resulted in a 30% reduced risk of AD, particularly if it was used for ≥10 years. Additionally, opposed HT initiated after the age of 70 years increased the risk of AD. HT use has also been shown to affect neural activation patterns (62). HT initiated at perimenopause and utilized continuously was associated with increased activation in left hippocampus and decreased bilateral activation in parahippocampal gyrus, compared with HT never used. These patterns were associated with improved memory performance on a task performed in the fMRI scanner (47).
Randomized Controlled Trials
Although favorable results have been obtained from randomized controlled trials investigating the effects of HT on cognition, the evidence has been mixed. Combination HT (either norethisterone and E2 or tibolone) was associated with improved semantic memory compared with placebo (63). Similarly, in a crossover study of early menopausal women taking CEE or placebo, participants on CEE exhibited better oral reading and semantic memory compared with control (64). Consistent with these results, compared with placebo, 12 weeks of E2 was associated with improved verbal memory in healthy postmenopausal women (65). Improvements in verbal memory (and very mild declines in auditory attention) were also observed among postmenopausal women randomized to 6 months of E2 valerate plus norethisterone versus placebo (66). Sherwin and Grigorova (67) found that whereas CEE plus MPA was unassociated with any cognitive change after a 3-month trial, CEE plus MP resulted in a mild (not clinically significant) decline in verbal memory and a significant improvement in working memory. Conversely, 48 weeks of E2 valerate plus placebo was associated with increased immediate verbal memory but decline in simple attention compared with E2 plus testosterone (68). HT has also been shown to be beneficial among women with AD. Short-term high-dose E2 was associated with significantly improved verbal memory, visual memory and attention in older postmenopausal women with AD (69). Furthermore, 3 months of HT (both opposed and unopposed) was associated with improvements in visual memory among postmenopausal women with moderate AD (70).
Findings from two randomized controlled trials of E2 versus placebo produced equivocal results. In one crossover study, transdermal E2 for 12 weeks resulted in cognitive benefit only in faster reaction time, whereas performance on measures of verbal and visual memory, attention, and working memory did not differ by group (71). Another study found that E2 was associated with greater improvement on immediate face recall, whereas the placebo group demonstrated stronger verbal memory and fluency (72). Two other studies did not display any group differences (E2 and E2 plus MP compared with placebo; testosterone or oral E2 compared with placebo) on cognitive measures (50, 73). Finally, the Cognitive Complaints in Early Menopause Trial (COGENT) study revealed no cognitive benefit of Prempro compared with placebo; indeed there was a close to significant mild decline in verbal memory in the HT group (74). The potential reasons for divergent findings reflecting methodological shortcomings are discussed in the previous sections.
Hormone therapy and mood/emotion
The higher prevalence of depressive disorders in women than men has been well established (35), and sex hormones contribute to this differential disease burden. Although not all women experience mood changes, for some women, substantial changes in estrogen during physiologic transitions such as pregnancy and menopause are particularly associated with worsening of depressive symptoms. Although the precipitous drop in estrogen levels during the menopausal transition is associated with higher depression rates, the evidence for treatment of depressive symptoms with exogenous estrogen remains unclear (37).
Observational Studies
Although results of observational studies have been mixed, recent investigations have not found positive associations between HT and improved mood. Early cross-sectional studies demonstrated correlations between estrogen use and reductions in depressive symptoms (75), but later research has not supported these findings. Use of HT in the previous month was associated with worse depression and anxiety among 6,000 peri- and postmenopausal women. No differences in mood were found based on type of estrogen preparation (76). Amore et al. (77) found that compared with perimenopause, postmenopausal women experienced higher levels of depression and sexual symptoms, and that HT (type not specified) was not associated with any change in mood. Neither HT (type not specified) nor menopausal status was associated with additional mood improvement in women taking antidepressant medication (78). Moreover, postmenopausal women taking HT for 3 months (either estrogen alone or estrogen plus progesterone) did not exhibit any change in mood when evaluated cross-sectionally. However, a separate group of 18 women taking HT (type not specified) who were followed prospectively for 3 months did demonstrate improvements in mood (79). Importantly, because hormone preparation was often not indicated, it is difficult to evaluate the degree to which type of estrogen or route of administration may have contributed to results.
Randomized Controlled Trials
In contrast, findings from clinical trials support a positive effect of estrogen on mood. Despite negative findings from one small single-blind study in which 14 women randomized to two forms of HT revealed no effect of either drug on mood after 6 months (63), five recent randomized placebo-controlled trials demonstrated significant beneficial associations between HT and mood. Eighty percent of women randomized to E2 reported significantly decreased mood symptoms after 3 or 6 weeks, compared with only 22% of women on placebo (80). Similarly, E2 resulted in improved mood in 68% of perimenopausal women with depressive disorders, whereas only 20% of women on placebo experienced similar benefit (81). Onalan et al. (82) prospectively examined the effects of three preparations of HT (CEE plus 2.5 mg MPA, CEE plus 5 mg MPA, or 2.5 mg tibolone) on mood over a 12-month period. Results showed that all three preparations improved depressive symptoms in menopausal women compared with placebo. In a randomized placebo-controlled cross-over trial, transdermal E2 was associated with significantly improved psychological well-being compared with placebo (71). Moreover, administration of transdermal E2 with or without norethisterone over 3 months was associated with significantly improved scores on anxiety and depression screening measures (83). Taken together, the data suggest that for the subset of women who experience depression associated with menopause, HT may be beneficial in addressing mood symptoms.
Shortcomings of the clinical trials to date
Dose, timing of HT initiation, duration of HT, and type of HT preparation are all important factors to consider in interpreting study results. Much of the research demonstrating no benefit of HT on cognition and mood has been marred by methodologic flaws or inconsistencies. Many studies were small, initiated hormone treatment >5 years after the menopausal transition, or used a less potent form of estrogen (e.g., CEE), a small dose, or a brief duration of treatment. As noted earlier, the effects of progesterone (especially MPA) are generally antagonistic to those of estrogen, and studies of cognition and affect using opposed HT display weaker results. Route of administration also affects outcomes. Moreover, there has been little attempt to mimic the premenopausal hormonal milieu, in which a complex array of hormones interact temporally and synergistically to promote cognitive, affective, and physical function. Steady-state HT may produce effects differently from a cyclic pattern (46). Although inconsistencies in findings remain, trials that address these methodologic considerations remain promising.
Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Study (KEEPS)
One of the studies that addresses several methodologic short-comings of earlier trials is the Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Study (KEEPS) and its ancillary study, the KEEPS Cognitive and Affective Study (KEEPS-Cog). All women enrolled in the clinical trial are nonhysterectomized, ≤3 years since their last menstrual period, and are at low risk for cardiovascular disease. The KEEPS-Cog will evaluate the efficacy of 4 years of therapy with oral CEE and transdermal E2 on mood and cognition. Micronized progesterone or matching placebo is administered cyclically, with dosing occurring during the first 12 days of the 30-day cycle. The parent KEEPS will examine surrogate markers of cardiovascular disease, osteoporosis, lipid profile, and coagulation cascade in this group of healthy, recently menopausal women. The findings of this study will test many of the theoretical explanations offered since the WHI study. For example, the long-term follow-up (anticipated for 20–25 years) of the KEEPS cohort will provide the opportunity to evaluate whether HT initiated at menopause may reduce the incidence of neurodegenerative diseases such as AD at later ages (4, 54).
Conclusion
The relationship between estrogen, cognition, and mood is complex. HT has not yet succeeded in achieving a postmenopausal hormonal milieu that approaches the intricate and dynamic premenopausal interactions between reproductive hormones and their multiple target systems. Despite negative findings, which, however, are marred by methodologic concerns, evidence continues to mount regarding the beneficial effect of HT, especially transdermal E2 and MP, on cognitive and affective function. Like KEEPS, future trials should systematically address inconsistencies in methodology to accurately characterize HT's effects. The potential for benefit is large. In addition to the possibility of increasing postmenopausal cognitive and affective function, HT may eventually represent a means to delay or even prevent neurodegenerative diseases.
References
- United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs PD. Population and Vital Statistics Report. Table 3 2007. Available at: http://unstats.un.org/UNSD/demographic/sconcerns/mortality/mort2.htm. Accessed February 3, 2014.
- Genazzani, A.R., Pluchino, N., Luisi, S., and Luisi, M. Estrogen, cognition and female ageing. Hum Reprod Update. 2007; 13: 175–187
- Coelingh Bennink, H.J. Are all estrogens the same?. Maturitas. 2004; 47: 269–275
- Gleason, C.E., Carlsson, C.M., Johnson, S., Atwood, C., and Asthana, S. Clinical pharmacology and differential cognitive efficacy of estrogen preparations. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2005; 1052: 93–115
- Asthana, S. Estrogen and cognition: a true relationship?. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2004; 52: 316–318
- Rannevik, G., Jeppsson, S., Johnell, O., Bjerre, B., Laurell-Borulf, Y., and Svanberg, L. A longitudinal study of the perimenopausal transition: altered profiles of steroid and pituitary hormones, SHBG and bone mineral density. Maturitas. 1995; 21: 103–113
- Irwin, R.W., Yao, J., Hamilton, R.T., Cadenas, E., Brinton, R.D., and Nilsen, J. Progesterone and estrogen regulate oxidative metabolism in brain mitochondria. Endocrinology. 2008; 149: 3167–3175
- Brinton, R.D. Estrogen regulation of glucose metabolism and mitochondrial function: therapeutic implications for prevention of Alzheimer's disease. Adv Drug Deliv Rev. 2008; 60: 1504–1511
- Brinton, R.D. The healthy cell bias of estrogen action: mitochondrial bioenergetics and neurological implications. Trends Neurosci. 2008; 31: 529–537
- Bailey, M.E., Wang, A.C., Hao, J., Janssen, W.G., Hara, Y., Dumitriu, D. et al. Interactive effects of age and estrogen on cortical neurons: implications for cognitive aging. Neuroscience. 2011; 191: 148–158
- Hao, J., Rapp, P.R., Janssen, W.G., Lou, W., Lasley, B.L., Hof, P.R. et al. Interactive effects of age and estrogen on cognition and pyramidal neurons in monkey prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007; 104: 11465–11470
- Luine, V.N., Wallace, M.E., and Frankfurt, M. Age-related deficits in spatial memory and hippocampal spines in virgin, female Fischer 344 rats. Curr Gerontol Geriatr Res. 2011; 2011: 316386
- Luine, V. and Frankfurt, M. Interactions between estradiol, BDNF and dendritic spines in promoting memory. Neuroscience. 2013; 239: 34–45
- Srivastava, D.P. Two-step wiring plasticity—a mechanism for estrogen-induced rewiring of cortical circuits. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol. 2012; 131: 17–23
- Wang, A.C., Hara, Y., Janssen, W.G., Rapp, P.R., and Morrison, J.H. Synaptic estrogen receptor-alpha levels in prefrontal cortex in female rhesus monkeys and their correlation with cognitive performance. J Neurosci. 2010; 30: 12770–12776
- Green, P.S. and Simpkins, J.W. Neuroprotective effects of estrogens: potential mechanisms of action. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2000; 18: 347–358
- Solum, D.T. and Handa, R.J. Estrogen regulates the development of brain-derived neurotrophic factor mRNA and protein in the rat hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2002; 22: 2650–2659
- Stirone, C., Duckles, S.P., Krause, D.N., and Procaccio, V. Estrogen increases mitochondrial efficiency and reduces oxidative stress in cerebral blood vessels. Mol Pharmacol. 2005; 68: 959–965
- Ono, H., Sasaki, Y., Bamba, E., Seki, J., Giddings, J.C., and Yamamoto, J. Cerebral thrombosis and microcirculation of the rat during the oestrus cycle and after ovariectomy. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol. 2002; 29: 73–78
- Sunday, L., Osuna, C., Krause, D.N., and Duckles, S.P. Age alters cerebrovascular inflammation and effects of estrogen. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol. 2007; 292: H2333–H2340
- Johnson, A.B., Bake, S., Lewis, D.K., and Sohrabji, F. Temporal expression of IL-1beta protein and mRNA in the brain after systemic LPS injection is affected by age and estrogen. J Neuroimmunol. 2006; 174: 82–91
- Sribnick, E.A., Del Re, A.M., Ray, S.K., Woodward, J.J., and Banik, N.L. Estrogen attenuates glutamate-induced cell death by inhibiting Ca2+ influx through L-type voltage-gated Ca2+ channels.Brain Res. 2009; 1276: 159–170
- Nilsen, J. and Diaz Brinton, R. Mechanism of estrogen-mediated neuroprotection: regulation of mitochondrial calcium and Bcl-2 expression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003; 100: 2842–2847
- Bagetta, G., Chiappetta, O., Amantea, D., Iannone, M., Rotiroti, D., Costa, A. et al. Estradiol reduces cytochrome c translocation and minimizes hippocampal damage caused by transient global ischemia in rat. Neurosci Lett. 2004; 368: 87–91
- Prediger, M.E., Gamaro, G.D., Crema, L.M., Fontella, F.U., and Dalmaz, C. Estradiol protects against oxidative stress induced by chronic variate stress. Neurochem Res. 2004; 29: 1923–1930
- Chiueh, C., Lee, S., Andoh, T., and Murphy, D. Induction of antioxidative and antiapoptotic thioredoxin supports neuroprotective hypothesis of estrogen. Endocrine. 2003; 21: 27–31
- Dubal, D.B., Shughrue, P.J., Wilson, M.E., Merchenthaler, I., and Wise, P.M. Estradiol modulates Bcl-2 in cerebral ischemia: a potential role for estrogen receptors. J Neurosci. 1999; 19: 6385–6393
- Garcia-Segura, L.M., Cardona-Gomez, P., Naftolin, F., and Chowen, J.A. Estradiol upregulates Bcl-2 expression in adult brain neurons. Neuroreport. 1998; 9: 593–597
- Bake, S., Ma, L., and Sohrabji, F. Estrogen receptor-alpha overexpression suppresses 17beta-estradiol–mediated vascular endothelial growth factor expression and activation of survival kinases. Endocrinology. 2008; 149: 3881–3889
- Nordell, V.L., Scarborough, M.M., Buchanan, A.K., and Sohrabji, F. Differential effects of estrogen in the injured forebrain of young adult and reproductive senescent animals. Neurobiol Aging. 2003; 24: 733–743
- Tinkler, G.P., Tobin, J.R., and Voytko, M.L. Effects of two years of estrogen loss or replacement on nucleus basalis cholinergic neurons and cholinergic fibers to the dorsolateral prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex of monkeys. J Comp Neurol. 2004; 469: 507–521
- Dumas, J., Hancur-Bucci, C., Naylor, M., Sites, C., and Newhouse, P. Estrogen treatment effects on anticholinergic-induced cognitive dysfunction in normal postmenopausal women.Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006; 31: 2065–2078
- Dumas, J.A., Kutz, A.M., Naylor, M.R., Johnson, J.V., and Newhouse, P.A. Estradiol treatment altered anticholinergic-related brain activation during working memory in postmenopausal women.Neuroimage. 2012; 60: 1394–1403
- Henderson, J.A. and Bethea, C.L. Differential effects of ovarian steroids and raloxifene on serotonin 1A and 2C receptor protein expression in macaques. Endocrine. 2008; 33: 285–293
- Halbreich, U. and Kahn, L.S. Role of estrogen in the aetiology and treatment of mood disorders.CNS Drugs. 2001; 15: 797–817
- Epperson, C.N., Amin, Z., Ruparel, K., Gur, R., and Loughead, J. Interactive effects of estrogen and serotonin on brain activation during working memory and affective processing in menopausal women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012; 37: 372–382
- Wharton, W., Gleason, C.E., Olson, S.R., Carlsson, C.M., and Asthana, S. Neurobiological underpinnings of the estrogen-mood relationship. Curr Psychiatry Rev. 2012; 8: 247–256
- Chakravorty, S.G. and Halbreich, U. The influence of estrogen on monoamine oxidase activity.Psychopharmacol Bull. 1997; 33: 229–233
- Shumaker, S.A., Legault, C., Rapp, S.R., Thal, L., Wallace, R.B., Ockene, J.K. et al. Estrogen plus progestin and the incidence of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in postmenopausal women: the Women's Health Initiative Memory Study: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2003;289: 2651–2662
- Shumaker, S.A., Legault, C., Kuller, L., Rapp, S.R., Thal, L., Lane, D.S. et al. Conjugated equine estrogens and incidence of probable dementia and mild cognitive impairment in postmenopausal women: Women's Health Initiative Memory Study. JAMA. 2004; 291: 2947–2958
- Rapp, SR, Espeland, MA, Shumaker, SA, Henderson, VW, Brunner, RL, Manson, JE et al. Effect of estrogen plus progestin on global cognitive function in postmenopausal women: the Women's Health Initiative Memory Study: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2006; 289: 2663–2672
- Espeland, M.A., Rapp, S.R., Shumaker, S.A., Brunner, R., Manson, J.E., Sherwin, B.B. et al. Conjugated equine estrogens and global cognitive function in postmenopausal women: Women's Health Initiative Memory Study. JAMA. 2004; 291: 2959–2968
- Simpkins, J.W., Perez, E., Wang, X., Yang, S., Wen, Y., and Singh, M. The potential for estrogens in preventing Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia. Ther Adv Neurol Disord. 2009; 2: 31–49
- Hogervorst, E., Williams, J., Budge, M., Riedel, W., and Jolles, J. The nature of the effect of female gonadal hormone replacement therapy on cognitive function in post-menopausal women: a meta-analysis. Neuroscience. 2000; 101: 485–512
- Grady, D., Yaffe, K., Kristof, M., Lin, F., Richards, C., and Barrett-Connor, E. Effect of postmenopausal hormone therapy on cognitive function: the Heart and Estrogen/Progestin Replacement Study. Am J Med. 2002; 113: 543–548
- Maki, P.M. Critical window hypothesis of hormone therapy and cognition: a scientific update on clinical studies. Menopause. 2013; 20: 695–709
- Maki, P.M., Dennerstein, L., Clark, M., Guthrie, J., LaMontagne, P., Fornelli, D. et al. Perimenopausal use of hormone therapy is associated with enhanced memory and hippocampal function later in life. Brain Res. 2011; 1379: 232–243
- MacLennan, A.H., Henderson, V.W., Paine, B.J., Mathias, J., Ramsay, E.N., Ryan, P. et al. Hormone therapy, timing of initiation, and cognition in women aged older than 60 years: the REMEMBER pilot study. Menopause. 2006; 13: 28–36
- Erickson, K.I., Voss, M.W., Prakash, R.S., Chaddock, L., and Kramer, A.F. A cross-sectional study of hormone treatment and hippocampal volume in postmenopausal women: evidence for a limited
- Kocoska-Maras, L., Zethraeus, N., Rådestad, A.F., Ellingsen, T., von Schoultz, B., Johannesson, M. et al.A randomized trial of the effect of testosterone and estrogen on verbal fluency, verbal memory, and spatial ability in healthy postmenopausal women. Fertil Steril. 2011; 95: 152–157
- Henderson, V.W., St. John, J.A., Hodis, H.N., McCleary, C.A., Stanczyk, F.Z., Karim, R. et al. Cognition, mood, and physiological concentrations of sex hormones in the early and late postmenopause.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013; 110: 20290–20295
- Gleason, C.E., Dowling, N.M., Friedman, E., Wharton, W., and Asthana, S. Using predictors of hormone therapy use to model the healthy user bias: how does healthy user status influence cognitive effects of hormone therapy?. Menopause. 2012; 19: 524–533
- Hogervorst, E. Effects of gonadal hormones on cognitive behavior in elderly men and women. J Neuroendocrinol. 2013; 5: 1182–1195
- Wharton, W., Gleason, C.E., Miller, V.M., and Asthana, S. Rationale and design of the Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Study (KEEPS) and the KEEPS Cognitive and Affective Sub Study (KEEPS Cog). Brain Res. 2013; 1514: 12–17
- Hogervorst, E., Yaffe, K., Richards, M., and Huppert, F.A. Hormone replacement therapy to maintain cognitive function in women with dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2009; : CD003799
- Weissberger, A.J., Ho, K.K., and Lazarus, L. Contrasting effects of oral and transdermal routes of estrogen replacement therapy on 24-hour growth hormone (GH) secretion, insulin-like growth factor I, and GH-binding protein in postmenopausal women. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1991; 72: 374–381
- Brinton, R.D., Thompson, R.F., Foy, M.R., Baudry, M., Wang, J., Finch, C.E. et al. Progesterone receptors: form and function in brain. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2008; 29: 313–339
- Walf, A.A., Paris, J.J., Rhodes, M.E., Simpkins, J.W., and Frye, C.A. Divergent mechanisms for trophic actions of estrogens in the brain and peripheral tissues. Brain Res. 2011; 1379: 119–136
- de Lignières, B. Oral micronized progesterone. Clin Ther. 1999; 21: 41–60
- Buckwalter, JG, Crooks, VC, Robins, SB, and Petitti, DB. Hormone use and cognitive performance in women of advanced age. JAGS. 2004; 52: 182–186
- Zandi, P.P., Carlson, M.C., Plassman, B.L., Welsh-Bohmer, K.A., Mayer, L.S., Steffens, D.C. et al.Hormone replacement therapy and incidence of Alzheimer disease in older women: the Cache County Study. JAMA. 2002; 288: 2123–2129
- Shao, H., Breitner, J.C.S., Whitmer, R.A., Wang, J., Hayden, K., Wengreen, H. et al. Hormone therapy and Alzheimer disease dementia: new findings from the Cache County Study. Neurology. 2012; 79: 1846–1852
- Albertazzi, P., Natale, V., Barbolini, C., Teglio, L., and Di Micco, R. The effect of tibolone versus continuous combined norethisterone acetate and oestradiol on memory, libido and mood of postmenopausal women: a pilot study. Maturitas. 2000; 36: 223–229
- Shaywitz, S.E., Naftolin, F., Zelterman, D., Marchione, K.E., Holahan, J.M., Palter, S.F. et al. Better oral reading and short-term memory in midlife, postmenopausal women taking estrogen. Menopause. 2003; 10: 420–426
- Joffe, H., Hall, J.E., Gruber, S., Sarmiento, I.A., Cohen, L.S., Yurgelun-Todd, D. et al. Estrogen therapy selectively enhances prefrontal cognitive processes: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study with functional magnetic resonance imaging in perimenopausal and recently postmenopausal women. Menopause. 2006; 13: 411–422
- Alhola, P., Tuomisto, H., Saarinen, R., Portin, R., Kalleinen, N., and Polo-Kantola, P. Estrogen + progestin therapy and cognition: a randomized placebo-controlled double-blind study. J Obstet Gynaecol Res. 2010; 36: 796–802
- Sherwin, B.B. and Grigorova, M. Differential effects of estrogen and micronized progesterone or medroxyprogesterone acetate on cognition in postmenopausal women. Fertil Steril. 2011; 96: 399–403
- Möller, M.C., Bartfai, A.B., and Rådestad, A.F. Effects of testosterone and estrogen replacement on memory function. Menopause. 2010; 17: 983–989
- Asthana, S., Baker, L.D., Craft, S., Stanczyk, F.Z., Veith, R.C., Raskind, M.A. et al. High-dose estradiol improves cognition for women with AD: results of a randomized study. Neurology. 2001; 57: 605–612
- Wharton, W., Baker, L.D., Gleason, C.E., Dowling, M., Barnet, J.H., Johnson, S. et al. Short-term hormone therapy with transdermal estradiol improves cognition for postmenopausal women with Alzheimer's disease: results of a randomized controlled trial. J Alzheimers Dis. 2011; 26: 495–505
- Schiff, R., Bulpitt, C.J., Wesnes, K.A., and Rajkumar, C. Short-term transdermal estradiol therapy, cognition and depressive symptoms in healthy older women. A randomised placebo controlled pilot cross-over study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005; 30: 309–315
- Almeida, O.P., Lautenschlager, N.T., Vasikaran, S., Leedman, P., Gelavis, A., and Flicker, L. A 20-week randomized controlled trial of estradiol replacement therapy for women aged 70 years and older: effect on mood, cognition and quality of life. Neurobiol Aging. 2006; 27: 141–149
- Wolf, O.T., Heinrich, A.B., Hanstein, B., and Kirschbaum, C. Estradiol or estradiol/progesterone treatment in older women: no strong effects on cognition. Neurobiol Aging. 2005; 26: 1029–1033
- Maki, P.M., Gast, M.J., Vieweg, A.J., Burriss, S.W., and Yaffe, K. Hormone therapy in menopausal women with cognitive complaints: a randomized, double-blind trial. Neurology. 2007; 69: 1322–1330
- Whooley, M.A., Grady, D., and Cauley, J.A. Postmenopausal estrogen therapy and depressive symptoms in older women. J Gen Intern Med. 2000; 15: 535–541
- Toffol, E., Heikinheimo, O., and Partonen, T. Associations between psychological well-being, mental health, and hormone therapy in perimenopausal and postmenopausal women: results of two population-based studies. Menopause. 2013; 20: 1–10
- Amore, M., di Donato, P., Berti, A., Palareti, A., Chirico, C., Papalini, A. et al. Sexual and psychological symptoms in the climacteric years. Maturitas. 2007; 56: 303–311
- Kornstein, S.G., Toups, M., Rush, A.J., Wisniewski, S.R., Thase, M.E., Luther, J. et al. Do menopausal status and use of hormone therapy affect antidepressant treatment response? Findings from the Sequenced Treatment Alternatives to Relieve Depression (STAR*D) study. J Womens Health (Larchmt). 2013; 22: 121–131
- Stephens, C, Bristow, V, and Pachana, NA. HRT and everyday memory at menopause: a comparison of two samples of mid-aged women. Women Health. 2006; 43: 37–57
- Schmidt, P.J., Nieman, L., Danaceau, M.A., Tobin, M.B., Roca, C.A., Murphy, J.H. et al. Estrogen replacement in perimenopause-related depression: a preliminary report. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2000;183: 414–420
- Soares, C.N., Almeida, O.P., Joffe, H., and Cohen, L.S. Efficacy of estradiol for the treatment of depressive disorders in perimenopausal women: a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001; 58: 529–534
- Onalan, G., Onalan, R., Selam, B., Akar, M., Gunenc, Z., and Topcuoglu, A. Mood scores in relation to hormone replacement therapies during menopause: a prospective randomized trial. Tohoku J Exp Med. 2005; 207: 223–231
- Karsidag, C., Karsidag, A., Buyukbayrak, E., Kars, B., Pirimoglu, M., Unal, O. et al. Comparison of effects of two different hormone therapies on mood and symptomatic postmenopausal women.Arch Neuropsychiatry. 2012; 49: 39–43